Skip to main content
Log in

Is the three-term contingency trial a predictor of effective instruction?

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments are reported which test the effect of increased three-term contingency trials on students' correct and incorrect math responses. Experiment 1 included two junior high school students in need of special instruction in mathematics while in Experiment 2 two other students with similar instructional needs from the same classroom were studied. In the first experiment the increased rate of presentation (3 times the baseline rate) was done without regard to whether the antecedent teacher presentations to the student were presented vocally or in written form; in the second experiment, the rates of presentation were systematically rotated across written and vocal presentations. The dependent variables were rate per minute of correct and incorrect responses of students (vocal or written). The experiments used designs incorporating features of the multiple baseline (Students A and B) and reversal (Students B, C, and D). The data showed that increasing the number of three-term trials increased correct rates while incorrect rates remained relatively low. The second experiment replicated these findings with two other students and found that the treatment effect occurred independently with vocal and written antecedents when each was isolated. The results warrant further research to test whether or not rates of presentation of three-term contingency trials are predictors of effective instruction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Carnine, D. W. (1976). Effects of two teacher-presentation rates on off-task behavior, answering correctly, and participation.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 199–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eicholz, R. E., O'Daffer, P. G., & Fleenor, C. R. (1985).Addison-Wesley mathematics series. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D. (1991). The teacher as strategic scientist: A solution to our educational crisis?Behavior and Social Issues, 1(2).

  • Greer, R. D., McCorkle, N., & Williams, G. (1989). A sustained analysis of the behaviors of schooling.Behavioral Residential Treatment, 4, 113–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, R. V., Delquadri, J., Greenwood, C. R., & Thurston, L. (1982). The importance of opportunity to respond in children's academic success. In E. B. Edgar, N. G. Haring, J. R. Jenkins, & E. E. Pious (Eds.),Mentally handicapped children: Education and training (pp. 58–88). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingham, P., & Greer, R. D. (in press). Supervisor effects on students and teachers behaviors in observed and generalized settings.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis.

  • Holt School Mathematics Series. (1974). Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

  • Jastak, J., Bijou, S., & Jastak, S. (1978).Wide range achievement test. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, R. L., Dunlap, G., & Dyer, K. (1980). Intertriai duration and learning in autistic children.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 91–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loos, F. M., Williams, K. P., & Bailey, J. S. (1977). A multielement analysis of the effect of teacher aides in an “open-style” classroom.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 437–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purkey, S. C., & Smith, M. S. (1983). Effective schools: A review.Elementary School Journal, 83, 427–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1983). School effects on pupil progress: Research, findings and policy implications. In L. S. Shulman & G. Sykes (Eds.)The handbook of teaching and policy (pp. 25–94). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sainato, D. M., Strain, P. S., Lefebreve, D., & Rapp, N. (1987). Facilitating transition times with handicapped preschool children: a comparison between peer-mediated and antecedent prompt procedures.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 285–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuster, J. W., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M. P., & Guiltinan, S. (1988). The effectiveness of a constant time-delay procedure to teach chained responses to adolescents with mental retardation.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 169–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1968).The technology of teaching. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stallings, J. (1980). Allocated academic time revisited or beyond time on task.Educational Researcher, 9(11), 11–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1986).Achieving educational excellence using behavioral strategies. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vargas, E. A., & Vargas, J. S. (1991). Programmed instruction: What it is and how to do it.Journal of Behavioral Education, 1(1).

  • Wechsler, D. (1974).Wechsler intelligence scale for children (revised). New York: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. (1987). Direct observation of academic learning time.Teaching Exceptional Children, 19(2), 13–17.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Albers, A.E., Greer, R.D. Is the three-term contingency trial a predictor of effective instruction?. J Behav Educ 1, 337–354 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00947188

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00947188

Key words

Navigation