Skip to main content
Log in

Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A sample of the literature on acquired dyslexia is reviewed with special reference to the nature of paralexic errors. Linguistic studies of six cases of dyslexia are presented, with appropriate neurological and psychological details; there are two cases of each of three hypothesized types of impairment, visual dyslyxia, “surface” (grapheme-phoneme) dyslexia, and “deep” (syntactico-semantic) dyslexia. Finally, a scheme for acquired dyslexia is proposed and related to the error patterns observed in “normal” adult readers and in children learning to read; our approach to lexical look-up is one that the diagram makers may have found sympathique.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alajouanine, Th., Lhermitte, F., and Ribaucourt-Ducarne, Bl. de (1960) Les alexies agnosiques et aphasiques. In Alajouanine, Th. (ed.),Les Grandes Activités du Lobe Occipital. Masson, Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anisfield, M., and Knapp, M. (1968). Association, synonymity, and directionality in false recognition.J. Exp. Psychol. 77, 171–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asayama, T. (1914). Über die Aphasie bei Japanern.Deut. Arch. Klin. Med. 113, 523–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, H. C. (1898).A Treatise on Aphasia and Other Speech Defects. H. K. Lewis, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson, D. F., and Geschwind, N. (1969). The alexias. In Vinken, P. J. and Bruyn, G. W. (eds.),Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 4 North-Holland, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benton, A. L. (1964). Contributions to aphasia before Broca.Cortex 1, 314–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berdiansky, B., Cronnell, B., and Koehler, J. (1969). Spelling-Sound Relations and Primary Form-Class Descriptions for Speech-Comprehension Vocabularies of 6–9 year-olds.Southwest Regional Laboratory for Educational Research and Development, Technical Report, No. 15.

  • Beringer, K., and Stein, J. (1930). Analyse eines Falles von “Reiner” Alexie.Z. Ges. Neurol. u. Psych. 123, 472–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T., and Bower, T. G. R. (1966): How to read without listening.Project Literacy Report No. 6, Cornell University, Ithaca, N.Y.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierwisch, M. (1970). Fehler-Linguistik.Linguistic Inquiry 1, 397–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, T. G. R. (1970). Reading by eye. In Levin, H. and Williams, J. P. (eds.),Basic Studies on Reading, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1967). The word frequency effect and response bias.Psych. Rev. 74, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., and Gregory, M. H. P. (1971). Effects on tachistoscopic perception from independent variation of word probability and of letter probability.Acta Psych. 35, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., and O'Dowd, D. (1958). A note on the informativeness of parts of words.Lang. Speech 1, 98, 101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casey, T., and Ettlinger, G. (1960). The occasional ‘independence’ of dyslexia and dysgraphia from dysphasia.J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 23, 228–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattlell, J. McKeen (1886). The inertia of the eye and brain.Brain 8, 295–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1979). Phonology and reading. In Levin, H. and Williams, J. P. (eds.),Basic Studies in Reading. Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. M. (1969). Reading errors and self-correction behavior.Brit. J. Educ. Psych. 39, 47–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crosland, H. R., and Johnson, G. (1928). The range of apprehension as affected by inter-letter hair-spacing and by the character of individual letters.J. Appl. Psych. 12, 82–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dale, H. C. A., and Gregory, M. (1966). Evidence of semantic coding in short-term memory.Psychon. Sci. 5, 75–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dejerine, J. (1892). Contribution a l'étude anatomopathologique et clinique des differents varietes de cecité verbale.Comp. Rend. Scéan. Biol. 4, 61–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubois-Charlier, F. (1971). Approche neurolinguistique du problème de l'aexie pure.J. Psych. 68, 39–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn-Rankin, P., and Leton, D. A. (1968). Hierarchical grouping procedures for comparing indices of letter similarity.Perceptual and Motor Skills 27, 457–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, W. (1897).Aphasia and the Cerebral Speech Mechanism. H. K. Lewis, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, W. (1900). The clinical varieties of visual aphasia.Edinburgh Med. J. 49, 433–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erdmann, B., and Dodge, R. (1898).Psychologische Untersuchungen uber das Lesen auf Experimenteller Grundlage. Neimeyer, Halle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franz, S. I. (1930). The relations of aphasia.J. Gen. Psych. 3, 401–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1891).Zur Auffassung der Aphasien. Deuticke, Leipzig and Vienna.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1962). The anatomy of acquired disorders of reading. In Money, J. (ed.),Reading Disorders. Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., and Guinet, L. (1971). Perception of inflections in brief visual presentations of words.J. Verb. Learn. Behav. 10, 182–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., Pick, A., Osser, H., and Hammond, J. (1962). The role of graphemephoneme correspondence in the perception of words.Amer. J. Psych. 75, 554–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., Shurcliff, A., and Yonas, A. (1970). Utilization of spelling patterns by deaf and hearing subjects. In Levin, H. and Williams, J. P. (eds.),Basic Studies in Reading, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, V. (1970). Factors affecting the comprehension of written English texts. Term Paper, Dept. of Linguistics, University of Edinburgh.

  • Gloning, I., Gloning, K., and Hoff, H. (1968).Neuropsychological Symptoms and Syndromes in Lesions of the occipital Lobe and the Adjacent Areas. Gauthier-Villars, Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldscheider, A., and Muller, R. F. (1893). Zur Physiologie und Pathologie des Lesens.Z. f. Klin. Med. 23, 131–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K. (1948).Language and Language Disturbances. Grune and Strastton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., and Hunter, M. (1970). A linguistic comparison of speech and writing in two types of aphasia.J. Commun. Dis. 3, 28–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, K. S. (1969). Analysis of oral reading miscues: applied psycholinguistics.Reading Res. Quart. 5, 9–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, H. (1965). Effects of stimulus variables on dysphasic verbal errors.Perceptual and Motor Skills,21, 292–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havens, L. L., and Foote, W. E. (1963). The effect of competition on visual duration threshold and its independence of stimulus frequency.J. Exp. Psych. 65, 6–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hécaen, H. (1967). Aspects des troubles de la lecture (alexies) au cours des lésions cérébrales on foyer.Word 23, 265–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, A., and Thorner, H. (1929). Experimentelle Untersuchungen zur Psychologie des Lesens.Arch. Ges. Psych. 71, 165–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henley, N. M., Noyes, H. L., and Deese, J. (1968). Semantic structure in short-term memory.J. Exper. Psych. 77, 587–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshelwood, J. (1900).Letter-, Word- and Mind-Blindness. H. K. Lewis, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, G. (1950). Pure word blindness.Folia Psychiat. Neurol. Neerl. 53, 279–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. M., Marshall, J. C., and Newcombe, F. (1971). Syntactic class as a determinant of word-retrieval in normal and dyslexic subjects.Nature (in press).

  • Holmes, J. M., Newcombe, F., and Marshall, J. C. (1971). “Literal” errors in acquired dyslexia. (Forthcoming).

  • Horowitz, L. M., Chilian, P. C., and Dunnigan, K. P. (1969). Word fragments and their redintegrative powers.J. Exp. Psych. 80, 392–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughlings-Jackson, J. (1878). On affections of speech from disease of the brain.Brain 1, 304–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ilg, F. L., and Ames, L. B. (1950). Developmental trends in reading.J. Genet. Psych. 76, 291–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Javal, E. (1881). L'evolution de la typographie.Rev. Sci. 27, 802–813.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M., and Warrington, E. K. (1962). A variety of reading disability associated with righ hemisphere lesions.J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 25, 339–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleist, J. (1934).Gehirn-Pathologie vornelnlich auf Grund der Kriegserfahrungen. Barth, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. (1966). Reading and talking bilingually.Amer. J. Psych. 79, 357–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. (1970). Three stages of reading. In Levin, H. and Williams, J. P. (eds.)Basic Studies in Reading Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuennapas, T. (1966). Visual perception of capital letters.Scand. J. Psych. 7, 189–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kussmaul, A. (1877).Die Störungen der Sprache. Vogel, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lhermitte, J., Lecours, A-R., and Ouvry, B. (1967). Essai d'analyse structurale des paralexies et des paragraphies.Acta Neurol. Psych. Belg. 67, 1021–1044.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1947).Traumatic Aphasia. English translation, 1970; Mouton, The Hague.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1960). Differences between disturbances of speech and writing in Russian and in French.Internat. J. Slavic Linguis. Poetics 3, 13–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. S., Kwan, S. T., and Chao, W. H. (1938). Left occipitoparietal brain tumor with observations on alexia and agraphia in Chinese and in English.The Chinese Med. J. 54, 491–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Makita, K. (1968). The rarity of reading disability in Japanese children.Amer. J. Orthopsych.,38, 599–614.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. C., and Newcombe, F. (1966). Syntactic and semantic errors in paralexia.Neuropsychologia 4, 169–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, M., Newcombe, F., and Marshall, J. C. (1970). The microstructure of word-finding difficulties in a dysphasic subject. In Flores d'Aracis, G.B. and Levelt, W. J. M. (eds.),Advances in Psycholinguistics. North-Holland, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Money, J., and Schiffman, G. (1966).The Disabled Reader: Education of the Dyslexic Child. Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1964). A model for continuous language behaviour.Lang. Speech 7, 40–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition.Psychol. Rev. 76, 165–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, F. (1969).Missile Wounds of the Brain: A Study of Psychological Deficits. Oxford University Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, F., and Marshall, J. C. (1971). Word-retrieval in aphasia: Work in progress. In Tzavaras, A. (ed.),Current Trends in European Human Neuropsychology. Special issue ofInternat. J. Ment. Health.

  • Orton, S. T. (1937).Reading, Writing and Speech Problems in Children, Chapman and Hall, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pieron, H. (1923).Le Cerveau et la Pensée. Alcan, Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poppelreuter, W. (1917).Die psychischen Schadigungen durch Kopfschuss in Kriege 1914–1916. Voss, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riegel, K. F., and Riegel, R. M. (1961). Prediction of word-recognition thresholds on the basis of stimulus parameters.Lang. Speech 4, 157–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Poritsky, S., and Sotsky, R. (1971). American children with reading problems can easily learn to read English represented by Chinese characters.Science 171, 1264–1267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, W. R., and Espir, M. L. E. (1961).Traumatic Aphasia. Oxford University Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanford, E. C. (1888). The relative legibility of the small letters.Amer. J. Psych. 1, 402–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sasanuma, S., and Fujimura, O. (1971). Selective impairment of phonetic and nonphonetic transcriptions of words in Japanese aphasic patients.Cortex 7, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuell, H. (1950). Paraphasia and Paralexia.J. Speech Hearing Dis. 15, 291–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, G. M. (1959). Dysphasic speech responses to visual word stimuli.J. Speech Hear. Res. 2, 152–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmel, M. L., and Goldschmidt, K. H. (1953). Prolonged posteclamptic aphasia.A.M.A. Archieves of Neurology and Psychiatry 69, 80–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F. (1969). The use of featural dependencies across letters in the visual identification of words.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 8, 215–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F. (1971).Understanding Reading. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomsen, I. V., and Harmsen, P. (1968). Retraining in a case of agnosic alexia.Folia Phoniat. 20, 342–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorner, H. (1929). Experimentelle Untersuchungen zur Psychologie des Lesens.Arch. Ges. Psych. 71, 127–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, M. D. (1929). The errors made in reading.Medical Research Council Reports of the Committee upon the Physiology of Vision. His Majesty's Stationery Office, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, M. D. (1931). Characteristics of proof reading.Brit. J. Psych. 21, 368–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, M. D. (1957).Backwardness in Reading. Cambridge University Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., and Zangwill, O. L. (1957). A study of dyslexia.J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 20, 208–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weigl, E., and Bierwisch, M. (1970). Neuropsychology and Linguistics: Topics of common research.Found. Lang 6, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wernicke, C. (1874).Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex. Cohn and Weigert, Breslau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, D. D. (1970). Processes in word recognition.Cog. Psych. 1, 59–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitaker, H. (1969).On the Representation of Language in the Human Brain. UCLA: Working Papers in Phonetics, No. 12.

  • Whitaker, H. (1971). Unsolicited nominalizations by aphasics: the plausibility of the Lexicalist model.Linguistics (in press).

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1938).Experimental Psychology, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zachrisson, R. (1931). Four hundred years of English spelling reform.Studia Neophilogica 4, 1–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeitler, J. (1900). Tachistoskopische Untersuchungen uber das Lesen.Philosophische Studien 16, 380–463.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Marshall, J.C., Newcombe, F. Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. J Psycholinguist Res 2, 175–199 (1973). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067101

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067101

Keywords

Navigation