Skip to main content
Log in

Food for creativity: tyrosine promotes deep thinking

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Anecdotal evidence suggests that creative people sometimes use food to overcome mental blocks and lack of inspiration, but empirical support for this possibility is still lacking. In this study, we investigated whether creativity in convergent- and divergent-thinking tasks is promoted by the food supplement l-Tyrosine (TYR)—a biochemical precursor of dopamine, which is assumed to drive cognitive control and creativity. We found no evidence for an impact of TYR on divergent thinking (“brainstorming”) but it did promote convergent (“deep”) thinking. As convergent thinking arguably requires more cognitive top-down control, this finding suggests that TYR can facilitate control-hungry creative operations. Hence, the food we eat may affect the way we think.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Acworth, I. N., During, M. J., & Wurtman, R. J. (1988). Tyrosine: effects on catecholamine release. Brain Research Bulletin, 21, 473–477.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akbari Chermahini, S., Hickendorff, M., & Hommel, B. (2012). Development and validity of a Dutch version of the remote associates task: an item-response theory approach. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 177–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akbari Chermahini, S., & Hommel, B. (2010). The (b)link between creativity and dopamine: spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition, 115, 458–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akbari Chermahini, S., & Hommel, B. (2012a). Creative mood swings: divergent and convergent thinking affect mood in opposite ways. Psychological Research, 76, 634–640.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akbari Chermahini, S., & Hommel, B. (2012b). More creative through positive mood? Not everyone! Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 319.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowden, E. M., Jung-Beeman, M., Fleck, J., & Kounios, J. (2005). New approaches to demystifying insight. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 322–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., Jongkees, B., Sellaro, R., & Hommel, B. (2013). Working memory reloaded: tyrosine repletes updating in the N-Back task. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 200. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00200.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., Jongkees, B. J., Sellaro, R., van den Wildenberg, W., & Hommel, B. (2014). Eating to stop: tyrosine supplementation enhances inhibitory control but not response execution. Neuropsychologia,. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.027.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create: the impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 116. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00116.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cools, R. (2006). Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function: implication for L-DOPA therapy in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 1–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cools, R., & D’Esposito, M. (2010). Dopaminergic modulation of flexible cognitive control in humans. In A. Björklund, S. Dunnett, L. Iversen, & S. Iversen (Eds.), Dopamine handbook (pp. 249–260). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood–creativity link: toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 739–756. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 656–669. doi:10.1177/0146167211435795.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deijen, J. B. (2005). Tyrosine. In: Lieberman, Kanarek & Prasad, Nutrition brain and behavior (pp. 363–381).

  • Deijen, J. B., & Orlebeke, J. F. (1994). Effect of tyrosine on cognitive function and blood pressure under stress. Brain Research Bulletin, 33, 319–323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deijen, J. B., Wientjes, C. J. E., Vullinghs, H. F. M., Cloin, P. A., & Langefeld, J. J. (1999). Tyrosine improves cognitive performance and reduces blood pressure in cadets after one week of a combat training course. Brain Research Bulletin, 48, 203–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • During, M. J., Acworth, I. N., & Wurtman, R. J. (1988). Effects of systemic l-tyrosine on dopamine release from rat corpus striatum and nucleus accumbens. Brain Research, 452, 378–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farage, M. A., Osborn, T. W., & MacLean, A. B. (2008). Cognitive, sensory, and emotional changes associated with the menstrual cycle: a review. Archives of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 278(4), 299–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feuerbach, L. A. (1862). Das Geheimnis des Opfers oder der Mensch ist was er ißt. In W. Bolin, F. Jodl, & X. Bd (Eds.), Ludwig Feuerbach Sämtliche Werke (pp. 41–67). Stuttgart-Bad Canstatt: Frommann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., & Hommel, B. (2012). Deep thinking increases task-set shielding and reduces shifting flexibility in dual-task performance. Cognition, 123, 303–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glaeser, B. S., Melamed, E., Growdon, J. H., & Wurtman, R. J. (1979). Elevation of plasma tyrosine after a single oral dose of l-tyrosine. Life Sciences, 25, 265–271. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(79)90294-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2013). Volition in action: intentions, control dilemmas and the dynamic regulation of intentional control. In W. Prinz, A. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science. Foundations of an emerging discipline. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2012). Convergent and divergent operations in cognitive search. In P. M. Todd, T. T. Hills, & T. W. Robbins (Eds.), Cognitive search: evolution, algorithms, and the brain (pp. 221–235). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Akbari Chermahini, S., van den Wildenberg, W., & Colzato, L.S. (2014). Cognitive control of convergent and divergent thinking: a control-state approach to human creativity. (Manuscript submitted).

  • Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Fischer, R., & Christoffels, I. (2011). Bilingualism and creativity: benefits in convergent thinking come with losses in divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 273.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 450–463. doi:10.1177/1745691612454134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., et al. (2004). Neural activity observed in people solving verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biology, 2(4), e97. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020097.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krug, R., Stamm, U., Pietrowsky, R., Fehm, H. L., & Born, J. (1994). Effects of menstrual cycle on creativity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19(1), 21–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, C. R., Castellani, J., Kramer, F. M., et al. (2007). Tyrosine supplementation mitigates memory decrements during cold exposure. Physiology and Behavior, 92(4), 575–582.

  • Markus, C. R., Firk, C., Gerhardt, C., Kloek, J., & Smolders, G. J. F. (2008). Effect of different tryptophan sources on amino acids availability to the brain and mood in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 201, 107–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of creative problem solving process. Psychological Review, 69, 200–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J. S., & Quenzer, L. F. (2005). Psychopharmacology: drugs, the brain, and behavior (pp. 4–7). Sunderland: Sinauer Associates. ISBN 0-87893-534-7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuis, S., Van Nieuwpoort, C., Veltman, D. J., & Drent, M. L. (2007). Effects of the noradrenergic agonist clonidine on temporal and spatial attention. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 193, 261–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nijstad, B. A., de Dreu, C. K. W., Rietzschel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21, 34–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. (2007). Creativity theories and themes: research, development and practice. Burlington: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., Weis, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: a single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shurtleff, D., Thomas, J. R., Schrot, J., Kowalski, K., & Harford, R. (1994). Tyrosine reverses a cold-induced working memory deficit in humans. Pharmacology Biochemestry and Behavior, 47, 935–941.

  • Thomas, J. R., Lockwood, P. A., Sing, A., & Deuster, P. A. (1999). Tyrosine improves working memory in a multitasking environment. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 64, 495–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research of L.S. Colzato is supported by a Vidi grant (#452-12-001) of the NWO (Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research). We thank our bachelor students, in particular Bonny Roos, for their enthusiasm and invaluable assistance in recruiting and testing the participants of this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lorenza S. Colzato.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Colzato, L.S., de Haan, A.M. & Hommel, B. Food for creativity: tyrosine promotes deep thinking. Psychological Research 79, 709–714 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0610-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0610-4

Keywords

Navigation