Skip to main content
Log in

Crabby commotions: visual not vibrational-orientated searching behaviours guide aggregation formation around key resources

  • Article
  • Published:
Journal of Ethology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Incidental stimuli produced by animal aggregations may be utilised by conspecifics to locate key resources in areas, where resources are patchily distributed and temporally irregular. However, few studies have investigated the roles of vibrational stimuli in aggregation formation when compared to other stimuli such as visual and tactile. Experimental manipulations were undertaken with Coenobita compressus, a terrestrial hermit crab of the tropics, which is constantly forming and re-forming aggregations around highly contested shell resources in a beach landscape. The commotions of jostling crabs, which may be up to hundreds of crabs at a time, produce a number of different incidental sensory stimuli during formation. Aspects of these stimuli were simulated, visual and vibrational, using playbacks and models. The visual simulation attracted significantly more crabs than the vibrational treatment, with the crabs also staying for a significantly longer period of time. The vibration simulation also did not attract more crabs when compared to a control, but visitors spent significantly less time in vibration-exposed quadrats, indicating reception of the stimulus. Taken together these findings indicate that visual public information promoted social aggregation formation, whereas vibrational information alone was inadequate.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  • Aicher B, Tautz J (1984) ‘Peripheral inhibition’ of vibration-sensitive units in the leg of the fiddler crab Uca pugilator. J Comp Physiol A 154:49–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball EE (1972) Observations on the biology of the hermit crab Coenobita compressus H. Milne Edwards (Decapoda; Anomura) on the west coast of the Americas. Rev Biol Trop 20:265–273

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates KM, Laidre ME (2018) When to socialize: perception of time-sensitive social structures among social hermit crabs. Anim Behav 138:19–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (2011) Principles of animal communication, 2nd edn. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Breithaupt T, Thiel M (2011) Chemical communication in crustaceans, 1st edn. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Caves EM, Brandley NC, Johnsen S (2018) Visual acuity and the evolution of signals. Trends Ecol Evol 33:358–372

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan A-H, Giraldo-Perez P, Smith S, Blumstein D (2010) Anthropogenic noise affects risk assessment and attention: the distracted prey hypothesis. Biol Lett 6:458–461

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chase ID, Weissburg M, Dewitt TH (1988) The vacancy chain process: a new mechanism of resource distribution in animals with application to hermit crabs. Anim Behav 36:1265–1274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cocroft RB (2001) Vibrational communication and the ecology of herbivorous insects. Am Zool 41:1215–1221

    Google Scholar 

  • Cocroft RB, Hamel J, Su Q, Gibson J (2014) Vibrational playback experiments: challenges and solutions. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (eds) Studying vibrational communication. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp 249–274

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Couzin I, Laidre ME (2009) Fission-fusion populations. Curr Biol 19:R633–R635

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronin TW, Johnsen S, Marshall NJ, Warrent EJ (2014) Visual ecology, 1st edn. Princeton Unviersity Press, Princeton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Danchin É, Giraldeau L-A, Valone TJ, Wagner RH (2004) Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305:487–491

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elias DO, Mason AC (2014) The role of wave and substrate heterogeneity in vibratory communication: practical issues in studying the effect of vibratory environments in communication. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (eds) Studying vibrational communication. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp 215–247

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fertin A, Casas J (2007) Orientation towards prey in antlions: efficient use of wave propagation in sand. J Exp Biol 210:3337–3343

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field LH, Evans A, MacMillan DL (1987) Sound production and stridulatory structures in hermit crabs of the genus Trizopagurus. J Mar Biol 67:89–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher LE, Yack JE, Fitzgerald TD, Hoy RR (2006) Vibrational communication in the cherry leaf roller caterpillar Caloptilia serotinella (Gracillarioidea: Gracillariidae). J Insect Behav 19:1–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Floyd RB, Woodland DJ (1981) Localization of soil dwelling scarab larvae by the black-backed magpie, Gymnorhina tibicen (Latham). Anim Behav 29:510–517

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harzsch S, Hansson BS (2008) Brain architecture in the terrestrial hermit crab Coenobita clypeatus (Anomura, Coenobitidae), a crustacean with a good aerial sense of smell. BMC Neurosci 9:1–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazlett B (1966) Observations on the social behavior of the land hermit crab, Coenobita clypeatus (Herbst). Ecology 47:316–317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill PSM (2008) Vibrational communication in animals. Harvard University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hill PSM, Wessel A (2016) Biotremology. Curr Biol 26:R187–R191

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horch K (1971) An organ for hearing and vibration sense in the ghost crab Ocypode. J Comp Physiol A 73:1–21

    Google Scholar 

  • Horch K (1975) The acoustic behavior of the ghost crab Ocypode cordimana Latreille, 1818 (Decapoda, Brachyura). Crustaceana 29:193–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horch K, Salmon M (1972) Responses of the ghost crab, Ocypode, to acoustic stimuli. Z Tierpsychol 30:1–13

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imafuku M, Ikeda H (1990) Sound production in the land hermit crab Coenobita purpureus Stimpson, 1858 (Decapoda, Coenobitidae). Crustaceana 58:168–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurta A (1982) Social faciliation of foraging behavior by the hermit crab Coenobita compressus, in Costa Rica. Biotropica 142:132–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laidre ME (2010) How rugged individualists enable one another to find food and shelter: field experiments with tropical hermit crabs. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 277:1361–1369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laidre ME (2012) Homes for hermits: temporal, spatial and structural dynamics as transportable homes are incorporated into a population. J Zool 288:33–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laidre ME (2013a) Foraging across ecosystems: Diet diversity and social foraging spanning aquatic and terrestrial ecosystems by an invertebrate. Mar Ecol 34:80–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laidre ME (2013b) Eavesdropping foragers use level of collective commotion as public information to target high quality patches. Oikos 122:1505–1511

    Google Scholar 

  • Laidre ME, Patten E, Pruitt L (2012) Costs of a more spacious home after remodelling by hermit crabs. J R Soc Interface 9:3574–3577

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis ER, Narins PM, Jarvis JUM, Bronner G, Mason MJ (2006) Preliminary evidence for the use of microseismic cues for navigation by the Namib golden mole. J Acoust Soc Am 119:1260–1268

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ligges U, Sebastian K, Mersmann O, Schnackenberg S (2018) tuneR: Analysis of music and speech. see https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=tuneR

  • Martin K, Quine D, Marler P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization: II tropical forest habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:291–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor PK (1992) Playback and studies of animal communication, 1st edn. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McNett GD, Luan LH, Cocroft RB (2010) Wind-induced noise alters signaler and receiver behavior in vibrational communication. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:2043–2051

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton ES (1975) The American society of naturalists ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am Nat 109:17–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osorno JL, Fernández-Casillas L, Rodríguez-Juárez C (1998) Are hermit crabs looking for light and large shells?: Evidence from natural and field induced shell exchanges. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 222:163–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Partan SR (2017) Multimodal shifts in noise: switching channels to communicate through rapid environmental change. Anim Behav 124:325–337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Partan S, Marler P (1999) Communication goes multimodal. Science 283:1272–1273

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ping X, Lee JS, Garlick D, Jiang Z, Blaisdell AP (2015) Behavioral evidence illuminating the visual abilities of the terrestrial Caribbean hermit crab Coenobita clypeatus. Behav Processes 118:47–58

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polajnar J, Eriksson A, Rossi Stacconi MV, Lucchi A, Anfora G, Virant-Doberlet M, Mazzoni V (2014) The process of pair formation mediated by substrate-borne vibrations in a small insect. Behav Processes 107:68–78

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rado R, Terkel J, Wollberg Z (1998) Seismic communication signals in the blind mole-rat (Spalax ehrenbergi): nelectrophysiological and behavioral evidence for their processing by the auditory system. J Comp Physiol A 183:503–511

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reese ES (1963) The behavioral mechanisms underlying shell selection by hermit crabs. Behaviour 21:78–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rittschof D (1980) Chemical attraction of hermit crabs and other attendants to simulated gastropod predation sites. J Chem Ecol 6:103–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts L (2021) Substrate-borne vibration and sound production by the land hermit crab Coenobita compressus during social interactions. J Acoust Soc Am 149(5):3261–3272

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts L, Laidre ME (2019) Get off my back: vibrational assessment of homeowner strength. Biol Lett 15:20180819

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts L, Cheesman S, Elliott M, Breithaupt T (2016) Sensitivity of Pagurus bernhardus (L.) to substrate-borne vibration and anthropogenic noise. J Exp Mar Bio Ecol 474:185–194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RStudio (2015) RStudio: integrated development for R. RStudio

    Google Scholar 

  • Saavedra I, Amo L (2018) Insectivorous birds eavesdrop on the pheromones of their prey. PLoS ONE 13:1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmon M (1971) Signal characteristics and acoustic detection by the fiddler crabs, Uca rapax and Uca pugilator. Physiol Zool 44:210–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmon M, Atsaides SP (1969) Sensitivity to substrate vibration in the fiddler crab, Uca pugilator. Anim Behav 17(Part 1):68–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmon M, Horch K (1973) Vibration reception in the fiddler crab, Uca minax. Comp Biochem Physiol A Physiol 44:527–541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimasaki L, Kitagawa K, Hernandez M, Blumstein DT (2012) Are blue land crabs (Cardisoma guanhumi) attracted to falling fruit? Acta Ethol 15:159–164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small MP, Thacker RW (1994) Land hermit crabs use odors of dead conspecifics to locate shells. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 182:169–182

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahlman WD, Chan AAY-H, Blumstein DT, Fast CD, Blaisdell AP (2011) Auditory stimulation dishabituates anti-predator escape behavior in hermit crabs (Coenobita clypeatus). Behav Processes 88:7–11

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele EP, Laidre ME (2019) Leaf me alone: visual constraints on the ecology of social group formation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 73:53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sueur J, Aubin T, Simonis C (2008) Seewave: a free modular tool for sound analysis and synthesis. Bioacoustics 18:213–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valone TJ, Templeton JJ (2002) Public information for the assessment of quality: a widespread social phenomenon. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 357:1549–1557

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Virant-Doberlet M, Mazzoni V, de Groot M, Polajnar J, Lucchi A, Symondson WOC, Čokl A (2014) Vibrational communication networks: eavesdropping and biotic noise. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (eds) Studying vibrational communication. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp 93–123

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Virant-Doberlet M, Kuhelj A, Polajnar J, Šturm R (2019) Predator-prey interactions and eavesdropping in vibrational communication networks. Front Ecol Evol 7:1–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward A, Webster M (2016) Sociality: the behaviour of group-living animals, 1st edn. Springer International Publishing

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Yack JE, Smith ML, Weatherhead PJ (2001) Caterpillar talk: acoustically mediated territoriality in larval Lepidoptera. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:11371–11375

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yadav C, Guedes RNC, Matheson SM, Timbers TA, Yack JE (2017) Invitation by vibration: recruitment to feeding shelters in social caterpillars. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 71:51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeil J, Hemmi JM (2006) The visual ecology of fiddler crabs. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 192:1–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thank you to staff at Osa Conservation Field Station (Costa Rica), and to C. Doherty, E. Steele, L. Valdes and J. Krieger for company in the field. A special thanks to the Dartmouth Women in Science Programme (WISP) undergraduate students A. Morris and P. Bazylczyk for their video coding. Thanks to G. Clucas and to the Shoals Marine Laboratory 2018 interns for island fun. The reviewers are thanked for providing valuable feedback. This work was supported by Dartmouth College (Hanover, NH), and a Shoals Marine Laboratory (UNH and Cornell University, NH, NY) Scientist-in-Residence Fellowship awarded to the author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Louise Roberts.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 4730 KB)

Supplementary file2 (MP4 28427 KB)

Supplementary file3 (MP4 23827 KB)

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Roberts, L. Crabby commotions: visual not vibrational-orientated searching behaviours guide aggregation formation around key resources. J Ethol 39, 343–351 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-021-00710-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-021-00710-5

Keywords

Navigation