Skip to main content
Log in

Children’s concept of probability as inferred from their binary choices—revisited

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Children had to choose one of two urns—each comprising beads of winning and losing colours—from which to draw a winning bead. Three experiments, aimed at diagnosing rules of choice and designed without confounding possible rules with each other, were conducted. The level of arithmetic difficulty of the trials was controlled so as not to distort the effects of the constituent variables of proportion. Children aged 4 to 11 first chose by more winning elements and proceeded with age to choices by greater proportion of winning elements. There were some indications of intermediate choices by fewer losing elements and by greater difference between the two colours. Distinguishing correct choices from favourable draws, namely acknowledging the role of chance in producing the outcome and insisting on the right choice, grew with age. Children switched rather early from considering one dimension to two; they combined the quantities of winning and losing elements either additively by difference or, with age, mostly multiplicatively by proportion. Guided playful activities for young children, based on this research, are suggested for fostering acquisition of the basic constituents of the probability concept: uncertainty of outcome, quantification by proportion, and the reverse relation between the chances of complementary events.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The apparent certainty of success following a correct choice in Noelting’s (1980a, b) task is due to the law of large numbers. The sampled beverage for drinking comprises multitude of molecules. Were the participants to draw a single molecule, the situation would reduce to that of drawing a bead from an urn.

  2. Quoted in Teaching Statistics (1985), 7, 92

  3. The drop in the functions for greater w at age 8 might be accidental or a case of the “U-shaped behavioral growth” in proportional reasoning, as observed by Stavy et al. (1982).

  4. Though Bayesian analysis implies some increase (decrease) of the confidence in the correctness of a choice following even one instance of positive (negative) feedback, this does not entail certainty.

  5. The story goes that Bertrand Russell commented on this apparent oxymoron: “How dare we speak of the laws of chance? Is not chance the antithesis of all law?”

References

  • Acredolo, C., & O’Connor, J. (1991). On the difficulty of detecting cognitive uncertainty. Human Development, 34(4), 204–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H., & Schlottmann, A. (1991). Developmental study of personal probability. In N. H. Anderson (Ed.), Contributions to information integration theory (Vol. III: Developmental) (pp. 110–134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J., & Hershey, J. C. (1988). Outcome bias in decision evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 569–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1981). Working memory and the developmental analysis of probability judgment. Psychological Review, 88(6), 463–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castro, C. S. (1998). Teaching probability for conceptual change. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 35, 233–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, R. H. (1975). The development of children’s understanding of proportions. Child Development, 46, 141–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58(1), 1–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, C. M. (1965). Development of the probability concept in children. Child Development, 36(3), 779–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, A. L., & Mollaison, M. (1986). Understanding and solving probability problems: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 23–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 819–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Even, R., & Kvatinsky, T. (2010). What mathematics do teachers with contrasting teaching approaches address in probability lessons? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 74, 207–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, R. (1978). Analysis of the concept of probability in young children. In E. Cohors-Fresenborg & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the Second International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 144–147). Osnabrück, Germany: Universität Osnabrück.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, R. (1983). Children’s choice behaviour in probabilistic situations. In D. R. Grey, P. Holmes, V. Barnett, & G. M. Constable (Eds.), Proceedings of the First International Conference on Teaching Statistics (Vol. 2, pp. 714–726). Sheffield, England: University of Sheffield, Teaching Statistics Trust.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, R. (2010). The infinite challenge: Levels of conceiving the endlessness of numbers. Cognition and Instruction, 28(1), 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, R., Falk, R., & Levin, I. (1980). A potential for learning probability in young children. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(2), 181–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, R., & Konold, C. (1992). The psychology of learning probability. In F. Gordon & S. Gordon (Eds.), Statistics for the twenty-first century (pp. 151–164). Washington, DC: Mathematical Association of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, R., & Wilkening, F. (1998). Children’s construction of fair chances: Adjusting probabilities. Developmental Psychology, 34(6), 1340–1357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1975). The intuitive sources of probabilistic thinking in children. Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics: An educational approach. Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E., & Gazit, A. (1984). Does the teaching of probability improve probabilistic intuitions? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 15, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E., Nello, M. S., & Marino, M. S. (1991). Factors affecting probabilistic judgements in children and adolescents. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22, 523–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E., Pampu, I., & Minzat, I. (1970). Comparison of ratios and the chance concept in children. Child Development, 41, 377–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E., & Schnarch, D. (1997). The evolution with age of probabilistic, intuitively based misconceptions. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 28(1), 96–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Gaissmaier, W., Kurz-Milcke, E., Schwartz, L. M., & Woloshin, S. (2008). Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(2), 53–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, S. (1966). Probability judgments by preschool children: Task conditions and performance. Child Development, 37, 157–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, A. S., & Kapadia, R. (1984). Children’s conceptions of probability—a psychological and pedagogical review. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 15, 349–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoemann, H. W., & Ross, B. M. (1971). Children’s understanding of probability concepts. Child Development, 42, 221–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoemann, H. W., & Ross, B. M. (1982). Children’s concepts of chance and probability. In C. J. Brainerd (Ed.), Children’s logical and mathematical cognition: Progress in cognitive development research (pp. 93–121). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, B. L., & Huber, O. (1987). Development of the concept of comparative subjective probability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 304–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. A., Langrall, C. W., Thornton, C. A., & Mogill, A. T. (1997). A framework for assessing and nurturing young children’s thinking in probability. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 32, 101–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. A., Langrall, C. W., Thornton, C. A., & Mogill, A. T. (1999). Students’ probabilistic thinking in instruction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(5), 487–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49–81). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kokis, J. V., Macpherson, R., Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Heuristic and analytic processing: Age trends and associations with cognitive ability and cognitive styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 26–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leron, U., & Hazzan, O. (2009). Intuitive vs. analytical thinking: Four perspectives. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 71, 263–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martignon, L., & Krauss, S. (2009). Hands-on activities for fourth graders: A tool box for decision-making and reckoning with risk. International Electronic Journal of Mathematics Education, 4(3), 227–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (2004). Cognition and chance: The psychology of probabilistic reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson, P. (2007). Different ways in which students handle chance encounters in the explorative setting of a dice game. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 66, 293–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noelting, G. (1980a). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept: Part I—differentiation of stages. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(2), 217–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noelting, G. (1980b). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept: Part II—problem-structure at successive stages; problem-solving strategies and the mechanism of adaptive restructuring. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(3), 331–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Offenbach, S. I., Gruen, G. E., & Caskey, B. J. (1984). Development of proportional response strategies. Child Development, 55, 963–972.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1979). Discrepant results in experimental studies of young children’s understanding of probability. Child Development, 50, 1121–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children (L. Leake Jr., P. Burrell & H. D. Fishbein, Trans.). New York: Norton (Original work published 1951).

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1994). The origins of probability judgment: A review of data and theories. In G. Wright & P. Ayton (Eds.), Subjective probability (pp. 239–272). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (2008). Numeracy, ratio bias, and denominator neglect in judgments of risk and probability. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 89–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlottmann, A., & Wilkening, F. (2011). Judgment and decision making in young children. In M. K. Dhami, A. Schlottmann, & M. Waldmann (Eds.), Judgement and decision making as a skill: Learning, development, and evolution (pp. 55–83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz, R. W., & Waschescio, R. (1986). Children’s cognitive strategies in two-spinner tasks. In L. Burton & C. Hoyles (Eds.), Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 1, pp. 463–468). London: University of London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaughnessy, J. M. (1977). Misconceptions of probability: An experiment with a small-group, activity-based, model building approach to introductory probability at the college level. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 8, 295–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., Waters, L. J., & Dinwiddy, L. S. (1988). Misleading children: Causal attributions for inconsistency under repeated questioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45(3), 438–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46(2, Serial No. 189).

  • Siegler, R. S. (1995). Children’s thinking: How does change occur? In F. E. Weinert & W. Schneider (Eds.), Memory performance and competencies: Issues in growth and development (pp. 405–430). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. A., & Resnick, L. B. (1992). Representations of proportional relationships: Are children part–part or part–whole reasoners? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 23(3), 231–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinillo, A. G., & Bryant, P. (1991). Children’s proportional judgments: The importance of “half”. Child Development, 62, 427–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinillo, A. G., & Bryant, P. E. (1999). Proportional reasoning in young children: Part–part comparisons about continuous and discontinuous quantity. Mathematical Cognition, 5(2), 181–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stavy, R., Strauss, S., Orpaz, N., & Carmi, G. (1982). U-shaped behavioral growth in ratio comparisons. In S. Strauss & R. Stavy (Eds.), U-shaped behavioral growth (pp. 11–36). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stavy, R., & Tirosh, D. (1996). Intuitive rules in science and mathematics: The case of ‘more of A–more of B’. International Journal of Science Education, 18(6), 653–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tourniaire, F., & Pulos, S. (1985). Proportional reasoning: A review of the literature. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 16(2), 181–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). From addition to multiplication … and back. The development of students’ additive and multiplicative reasoning skills. Cognition and Instruction, 28(3), 360–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yost, P. A., Siegel, A. E., & Andrews, J. M. (1962). Nonverbal probability judgments by young children. Child Development, 33, 769–780.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The study was partly supported by the Sturman Center for Human Development, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem. We are grateful to Raphael Falk for his invaluable help in all the stages of the research and to graduate students of the Hebrew University for their assistance in collecting data for Experiment 3. Yael Oren took care of the figures.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ruma Falk.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1

(DOC 502 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Falk, R., Yudilevich-Assouline, P. & Elstein, A. Children’s concept of probability as inferred from their binary choices—revisited. Educ Stud Math 81, 207–233 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-012-9402-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-012-9402-1

Keywords

Navigation