Skip to main content
Log in

Comparing and Integrating Biological and Cultural Moral Progress

  • Published:
Ethical Theory and Moral Practice Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Moral progress may be a matter of time scale. If intuitive measures of moral progress like the degree of physical violence within a society are taken as empirical markers, then most human societies have experienced moral progress in the last few centuries. However, if the development of the human species is taken as relevant time scale, there is evidence that humanity has experienced a global moral decline compared to a small-band hunter-gatherer (SBHG) baseline that represents a lifestyle presumed to largely account for 99% of human history. A counter-argument to such a diagnosis of moral decline is the fact that the living conditions of the modern world that emerged since sedentariness and the beginning of agriculture are completely different compared to those of SBHG due to cultural and technological developments. We therefore suggest that two notions of moral progress should be distinguished: a “biological notion” referring to the inherited capacities typical of the evolutionary niche of mammals and that unfold in a specific way in the human species; and a “cultural notion” that relates moral progress to dealing with an increasing diversity of temptations and possible wrongdoings in a human social world whose complexity accumulates in time. In our contribution, we describe these two different notions of moral progress, we discuss how they interact, how this interaction impacts the standards by which we measure moral progress, and we provide suggestions and justifications for re-aligning biological and cultural moral progress.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A concise overview on the philosophy of “progress” is available at the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/progress/ (last access: October 31 2015).

  2. We note that these analyses are disputed by experts on empirical and categorization grounds (see Ferguson 2013a, 2013b; Fry 2013). For example, small-band hunter-gatherers, who represent 99% of human genus history, are relatively peaceful and not war-like, but Pinker mixes them together with complex hunter-gatherers, who can be war-like.

  3. We note that human use of natural resources is not necessarily linked to the destruction of nature and decrease of biodiversity. For example, Europe today without human intervention would be wooded to a large degree, with a likely lower degree of biodiversity compared to a Europe with bounded and non-monocropping agricultural activity, because boundary zones between forest and acres increase the number of ecological niches.

  4. It is important to clarify that we do not use this term with a specific geographic focus (i.e., focus on Europe or North America) or racial implication (i.e., focus on “white” culture). The term “western” just denotes that the conceptual origin of the idea that humans are special in a normative sense and that this special status provides the legitimation of exploiting nature has its historic origin mainly in assumptions about human distinctiveness (Biblical theology) which undergird the rationalization of thinkers of the western world (e.g. Francis Bacon or René Descartes). As the example of contemporary China shows, exploitation of nature and environmental pollution are not bound to specific geographic regions or racial boundaries. And, as the example of the Easter Island civilization has shown, destruction of nature can also happen without the presence of a “western worldview” in a culture (Diamond 2005).

  5. This problem points to fundamental questions of moral philosophy: the quest for the universality of moral norms and the quest on how to “ground” ethical theories (foundationalism vs. coherentism). We will not outline these questions further.

  6. We do not claim that the biological systems determine the behavior of the agent, nor do we claim that we have complete knowledge how biological systems enable moral behavior – actually, an individual’s biological systems might be much more complex than initially thought. There are for example indications that even the type of microorganisms that populate a human body may have an effect on moral behavior (Kramer and Bressan 2015).

  7. With respect to indirect moral progress, one has to be aware that political declarations of progress are not the same as actual progress. For example, slavery is nowadays technically illegal throughout most of the world – but according to some global watch-dog groups (e.g., http://www.freetheslaves.net/), we still have millions of people that live under slave-like conditions. Thus, the removal of institutional racism may not address the persistence or growth of other, less institutionalized, forms of systematic oppression [we thank an anonymous reviewer for this observation].

  8. Another interesting question to ask would be: Why did these complex societies emerge, given the high “moral price” that resulted from this change in lifestyle? In addition, other problems resulted from this transition, for example a higher vulnerability for epidemics, decreased health from worse nutrition. The causes for this change are still a matter of scientific controversies; see for example Cochran and Harpending (2009).

  9. A similar argument has been put forward regarding nutrition. Some suggest that the human metabolism is not adapted to the modern feeding pattern, which would partly explain the increasing obesity problem in many countries once the “western” diet is introduced.

  10. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this observation.

  11. This information emerges from the HYDE (History Database of the Global Environment) database that presents (gridded) time series of population and land use for the last 12,000 years: http://themasites.pbl.nl/tridion/en/themasites/hyde/ (last access: October 31 2015).

  12. A highly topical instance of this disrespect towards others in children and adolescents is the systematic, targeted and ongoing power abuse characterizing bullying in both its normal (offline) and online forms (see for example Hymel et al. 2010 for an overview of the moral dimensions of bullying).

  13. However, too, peoples exist worldwide who have a legacy of or adopt the indigenous worldview.

References

  • Appiah KA (2010) The honor code: how moral revolutions happen. W.W. Norton & Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnsten AFT (2009) Stress signaling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(6):410–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arrows F, Narvaez D (2016) Reclaiming our indigenous worldview: a more authentic baseline for social/ecological justice work in education. In: McCrary N, Ross W (eds) Working for social justice inside and outside the classroom: a community of teachers, researchers, and activists. Peter Lang, New York, pp. 93–112

    Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri M (ed) (1972) Hunters and gatherers today. Prospect Heights, IL, Waveland

    Google Scholar 

  • Black DS, Sussman S, Unger JB (2010) A further look at the intergenerational transmission of violence: witnessing interparental violence in emerging adulthood. J Interpers Violence 25(6):1022–1042

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolin I (2010) Chillihuani’s culture of respect and the circle of courage. Reclaiming Children and Youth Worldwide 18(4):12–17

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University Of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan A (ed) (1979) The sociobiology debate. HarperCollins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Christen M, Alfano M (2013) Outlining the field – a research program for empirically informed ethics. In: Christen M, Fischer J, Huppenbauer M, Tanner C, van Schaik C (eds) Empirically informed ethics. Morality between facts and norms. Library of ethics and applied philosophy. Springer, Berlin, pp. 3–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Christen M, Müller S (2015) Effects of brain lesions on moral agency: ethical dilemmas in investigating moral behavior. In: Ohl F, Lee G, Illes J (eds) Ethical issues in Behavioural neuroscience. Current topics in behavioral neuroscience, vol 5. Springer, Berlin, pp. 159–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Christen M, Fischer J, Huppenbauer M, Tanner C, van Schaik C (eds) (2013) Empirically informed ethics. Morality between facts and norms. Library of ethics and applied philosophy. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Claeys G (2000) The “survival of the fittest” and the origins of social Darwinism. J Hist Ideas 61(2):223–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger CR, Salloum IM, Mezzich JE (2012) The dynamic origins of positive health and wellbeing. Int J Person Centered Med 2(2):179–187

    Google Scholar 

  • Cochran G, Harpending H (2009) The 10,000 Year explosion: how civilization accelerated human evolution. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper T (1998) A time before deception. Clear Light Publishers, Santa Fe

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871) The Descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, vol I & II. Princeton University Press, Princeton original: Murray, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Derber C (2013) Sociopathic society: a people’s sociology of the United States. Paradigm Press, Boulder, CO

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond J (2005) Collapse. How societies choose to fail or succeed. Penguin, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson B (2013a) Pinker’s list: exaggerating prehistoric war mortality. In: Fry DP (ed) War, peace and human nature. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 112–129

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson B (2013b) The prehistory of war and peace in Europe and the near east. In: Fry DP (ed) War, peace and human nature. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 191–241

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fry DP (2006) The human potential for peace: an anthropological challenge to assumptions about war and violence. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Fry DP (ed) (2013) War, peace and human nature. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez-Robles A, Hopkins WD, Schapiro SJ, Sherwood CC (2015) Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci 112(48):14799–14804

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gowdy J (1998) Limited wants, unlimited means: a reader on hunter-gatherer economics and the environment. Island Press, Washington DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman A (1997) The idea of decline in western history. Free Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Lamb ME (2005) Hunter-gatherer childhoods: evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. Aldine, New Brunswick

    Google Scholar 

  • Highwater J (1981) The primal mind. Meridian (Penguin), New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hymel S, Schonert-Reichl KA, Bonanno RA, Vaillancourt T, Rocke Henderson N (2010) Bullying and morality: understanding how good kids can behave badly. In: Jimerson SM, Swearer S, Espelage DL (eds) Handbook of bullying in schools. An international perspective. Routledge, New York, pp. 101–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen D (2016) The myth of human supremacy. Seven Stories Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Joyce R (2006) The evolution of morality. MIT Press, Cambridge MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimmerer RW (2013) Braiding Sweetgrass: indigenous wisdom, scientific knowledge and the teachings of plants. Milkweed Editions, Minneapolis

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher P (2011) The ethical project. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MA

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Konner M (2005) Hunter-gatherer infancy and childhood: the !Kung and others. In: Hewlett B, Lamb M (eds) Hunter-gatherer childhoods: evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. Transaction, New Brunswich, NJ, pp. 19–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer P, Bressan P (2015) Humans as superorganisms: how microbes, viruses, imprinted genes, and other selfish entities shape our behavior. Perspect Psychol Sci 10(4):464–481

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee RB, Daly R (eds) (2005) The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Merchant C (2003) Reinventing Eden: the fate of Eden in western culture. Routledge, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Naess A, Rothenberg D (1989) Ecology, community and lifestyle. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D (2008) Triune ethics: the neurobiological roots of our multiple moralities. New Ideas Psychol 26:95–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D (2013) The 99 percent – development and socialization within an evolutionary context: growing up to become “a good and useful human being.”. In: Fry D (ed) War, peace and human nature: the convergence of evolutionary and cultural views. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 643–672

    Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D (2014) Neurobiology and the development of human morality. Evolution, culture, and wisdom. W.W. Norton & Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D (2015) The co-construction of virtue: epigenetics, neurobiology and development. In: Snow NE (ed) Cultivating virtue. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 251–277

    Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D (2016a) Evolution, early experience and Darwin’s moral sense. In: Joyce R (ed) Routledge handbook of evolution and philosophy. Routledge, London (in press)

  • Narvaez D (2016b) Embodied morality: protectionism, engagement and imagination. Palgrave-Macmillan, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D, Gleason T, Wang L, Brooks J, Lefever J, Cheng A, Centers for the Prevention of Child Neglect (2013a) The evolved development niche: longitudinal effects of caregiving practices on early childhood psychosocial development. Early Child Res Q 28(4):759–773

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D, Wang L, Gleason T, Cheng A, Lefever J, Deng L (2013b) The evolved developmental niche and sociomoral outcomes in Chinese three-year-olds. Eur J Dev Psychol 10(2):106–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez D, Gettler L, Braungart-Rieker J, Miller L, Harris P (2016) Flourishing as an aim of child development. In: Narvaez D, Braungart-Rieker J, Miller L, Gettler L, Harris P (eds) Contexts for young child flourishing: evolution, family and society. Oxford University Press, New York, NY, pp. 3–27

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson MK (ed) (2008) Original instructions: indigenous teachings for a sustainable future. Bear & Company, Rochester, Vermont

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama S, Griffiths PE, Gray RD (2001) Cycles of contingency: developmental systems and evolution. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (2011) Better angels of our nature: why violence has declined. Penguin Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Powledge TM (2011) Behavioral epigenetics: how nurture shapes nature. Bioscience 61:588–592

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins M (2008) The western illusion of human nature. Prickly Pear Paradigm Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Spengler O (1918) Der Untergang des Abendlandes–Umrisse einer Morphologie der Weltgeschichte (Band 1: Gestalt und Wirklichkeit). Braumüller, Wien

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevathan WR (2011) Human birth: an evolutionary perspective. Aldine Transaction, Piscataway NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Schaik C, Burkart J, Jaeggi A, Rudolf von Rohr C (2013) Morality as a biological adaptation–an evolutionary model based on the lifestyle of human foragers. In: Christen M, Fischer J, Huppenbauer M, Tanner C, van Schaik C (eds) Empirically informed ethics. Springer, Berlin, pp. 65–84

    Google Scholar 

  • West MJ, King AP (1987) Settling nature and nurture into an ontogenetic niche. Dev Psychobiol 20(5):549–562

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Markus Christen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Christen, M., Narvaez, D. & Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E. Comparing and Integrating Biological and Cultural Moral Progress. Ethic Theory Moral Prac 20, 55–73 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10677-016-9773-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10677-016-9773-y

Keywords

Navigation