Skip to main content
Log in

Gotta Name’em All: an Experimental Study on the Sound Symbolism of Pokémon Names in Brazilian Portuguese

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Sound-symbolic patterns which relate to the perception of size were found to motivate the behavior of English and Japanese speakers in the naming of pre- and post-evolution Pokémon. The current study builds from this finding and investigates which sound-symbolic association speakers of Brazilian Portuguese (BP) employ to name Pokémon characters. Results from 3 experiments show that vowel quality, phonological length and voiced obstruents, usually used to signal differences in size, are used to signal differences in evolution; however, the effects of voiced obstruents are not identical to what was previously observed in the behavior of Japanese speakers. We argue that although there is a universal sound symbolism associated with these sounds and the perception of largeness, its manifestation differs cross-linguistically. To the best of our knowledge, this is one the first experimental research to investigate sound symbolism and the perception of size in BP.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Starr et al. (2018) did not report any finding for these variables.

  2. When a Pokémon undergoes evolution, size is not the only change it experiences: it becomes stronger, faster, and sometimes its looks becomes more aggressive (see Fig. 1). Of all these changing features, size is the one that can be visually perceived and can clearly show that two related Pokémon are the pre and post-evolution version of the same character. In our study, we made this difference even more striking by making the pictures of post-evolution Pokémon characters larger (“Materials and Procedures” section). For this reason, we take any effect that shows a correlation between sound and evolutionary status to be at least partially grounded on the size of these characters.

  3. https://t0t0mo.jimdo.com/. The pictures were used in the experiment with the permission of the artist.

  4. Our referees for the first experiment were the second and the fourth co-author of this paper, and an undergraduate student at the UFRN English Language program.

  5. As we mentioned, these studies are independent and differ in their methodology. The corpus for the study in Japanese was comprised of texts published in newspapers, while the study in BP used a dictionary. We use their results here to present an estimative of frequency for the voiced obstruents in these languages, but a thorough investigation of this issue would require a new and more balanced cross-linguistic corpus study.

References

  • Aryani, A., Jacobs, A. M., & Conrad, M. (2013). Extracting salient sublexical units from written texts: “Emophon”, a corpus-based approach to phonological iconicity. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00654.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Asano, M., & Yokosawa, K. (2011). Synesthetic colors are elicited by sound quality in Japanese synesthetes. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1816–1823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.05.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. Retrieved from JSTOR.

  • Berlin, B. (2006). The first congress of ethnozoological nomenclature. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 12(s1), S23–S44. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9655.2006.00271.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blasi, D. E., Wichmann, S., Hammarström, H., Stadler, P. F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2016). Sound–meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(39), 10818–10823. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1605782113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & Groot, A. M. B. D. (2005). Onset entropy matters—letter-to-phoneme mappings in seven languages. Reading and Writing, 18(3), 211–229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bremner, A. J., Caparos, S., Davidoff, J., de Fockert, J., Linnell, K. J., & Spence, C. (2013). “Bouba” and “Kiki” in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape-sound matches, but different shape-taste matches to Westerners. Cognition, 126(2), 165–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coulter, K. S., & Coulter, R. A. (2010). Small sounds, big deals: Phonetic symbolism effects in pricing. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 315–328. https://doi.org/10.1086/651241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cruz, R. C. F., & Fernandes, H. F. G. (2004). Simbolismo sonoro do PB: o estudo dos ideofones [Sound Symbolism in Brazilian Portuguese: a study of ideophones]. Revista de Estudos da Linguagem, 12(2), 439–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuskley, C. (2013). Mappings between linguistic sound and motion. Public Journal of Semiotics, 5(1), 39–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daland, R., Hayes, B., White, J., Garellek, M., Davis, A., & Norrmann, I. (2011). Explaining sonority projection effects. Phonology, 28(2), 197–234. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675711000145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 603–615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dingemanse, M., Schuerman, W. L., Reinisch, E., Tufvesson, S., & Mitterer, H. (2016). What sound symbolism can and cannot do: Testing the iconicity of ideophones from five languages. Language, 92(2), e117–e133. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2016.0034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dingemanse, M., Torreira, F., & Enfield, N. J. (2013). Is “Huh?” A universal word? Conversational infrastructure and the convergent evolution of linguistic items. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e78273. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078273.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ernestus, M., & Baayen, R. H. (2003). Predicting the unpredictable: Interpreting neutralized segments in Dutch. Language, 71(1), 5–38. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2003.0076.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fort, M., Martin, A., & Peperkamp, S. (2014). Consonants are more important than vowels in the bouba-kiki effect. Language and Speech, 58, 247–266. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830914534951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godoy, M., Duarte, A. C. V., da Silva, F. L. F., Albano, G. F., de Souza, R. J. P., & da Silva, Y U Ade P M. (2018). Replicando o efeito Takete-Maluma em Português Brasileiro. Revista do GELNE, 20(1), 87–100. https://doi.org/10.21680/1517-7874.2018v20n1ID13331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haiman, J. (1980). The iconicity of grammar: Isomorphism and motivation. Language, 56(3), 515–540. https://doi.org/10.2307/414448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haiman, J. (Ed.). (1985). Iconicity in syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamano, S. (1988). The sound symbolic system of Japanese. Doctoral dissertation, University of Florida.

  • Hay, J., Pierrehumbert, J., & Beckman, M. (2003). Speech perception, well-formedness, and the statistics of the lexicon. In J. Local, R. Ogden, & R. Temple (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VI: Phonetic interpretation (pp. 58–74). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirata, S., Ukita, J., & Kita, S. (2011). Implicit phonetic symbolism in voicing of consonants and visual lightness using Garner’s speeded classification task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 113(3), 929–940. https://doi.org/10.2466/15.21.28.PMS.113.6.929-940.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollard, M., & Wertheimer, M. (1964). Some physiognomic aspects of naming, or maluma and takete revisited. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19, 111–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2014). The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130298. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., Kita, S., Nagumo, M., & Okada, H. (2008). Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning. Cognition, 109(1), 54–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iwasaki, N., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2007a). How does it hurt, kiri-kiri or siku-siku? Japanese mimetic words of pain perceived by Japanese speakers and English speakers. In M. Minami (Ed.), Applying theory and research to learning Japanese as a foreign language (pp. 2–19).

  • Iwasaki, N., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2007b). What do English speakers know about gera-gera and yotayota? A cross-linguistic investigation of mimetic words for laughing and walking. Sekai no nihongo kyoiku [Japanese Language Education Around the Globe], 17, 53–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kavitskaya, D. (2018). On Russian Pokémonastics. Talk at the first Conference in Pokémonastics, May 2018, Keyo University: Japan.

  • Kawahara, S., & Kumagai, G. (2019). Expressing evolution in Pokémon names: Experimental explorations. Journal of Japanese Linguistics, 35(1), 3–38. https://doi.org/10.1515/jjl-2019-2002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawahara, S., Noto, A., & Kumagai, G. (2018). Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon names. Phonetica, 75(3), 219–244. https://doi.org/10.1159/000484938.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kawahara, S., & Shinohara, K. (2011). Phonetic and psycholinguistic prominences in pun for- mation: Experimental evidence for positional faithfulness. In W. McClure & M. den Dikken (Eds.), Japanese/Korean linguistics (Vol. 18, pp. 177–188). Stanford, CA: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Grady, J. (2015). Iconic inferences about personality: From sounds and shapes. In M. Hiraga, W. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara, & K. Akita (Eds.), Iconicity: East meets west (pp. 57–69). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Uchimoto, Y. (2008). A positional effect in sound symbolism: An experimental study. In Proceedings of the Japan Cognitive Linguistics Association 8, pp. 417–427. JCLA, Tokyo.

  • Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lockwood, G., & Dingemanse, M. (2015). Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01246.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lockwood, G., Dingemanse, M., & Hagoort, P. (2016). Sound-symbolism boosts novel word learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(8), 1274–1281. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madureira, S., & Camargo, Z. A. (2010) Exploring sound symbolism in the investigation of speech expressivity. In A. Botinis (Ed), ISCA, 2010, Athens. Athens, vol. 01. pp. 105–108.

  • Maglio, S. J., Rabaglia, C. D., Feder, M. A., Krehm, M., & Trope, Y. (2014). Vowel sounds in words affect mental construal and shift preferences for targets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1082–1096. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D., Pathman, T., & Mondloch, C. J. (2006). The shape of boubas: Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science, 9(3), 316–322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moos, A., Smith, R., Miller, S. R., & Simmons, D. R. (2014). Cross-modal associations in synaesthesia: Vowel colours in the ear of the beholder. I-Perception, 5(2), 132–142. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0626.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, E. S. (1994). Sound symbolism and its role in non-human vertebrate communication. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols, & J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 348–365). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, S. (1933). Further experiments on phonetic symbolism. American Journal of Psychology, 45, 53–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, A. K. S., & Rendall, D. (2013). Parsing the role of consonants versus vowels in the classic Takete-Maluma phenomenon. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(2), 153–163. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nooteboom, S. (1981). Lexical retrieval from fragments of spoken words: Beginnings vs. endings. Journal of Phonetics, 9(4), 407–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nygaard, L. C., Cook, A. E., & Namy, L. L. (2009). Sound to meaning correspondences facilitate word learning. Cognition, 112(1), 181–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.04.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohala, J. J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica, 41(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohala, J. J. (1994). The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols, & J. J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound Symbolism (pp. 325–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohtake, Y., & Haryu, E. (2013). Investigation of the process underpinning vowel-size correspondence. Japanese Psychological Research, 55(4), 390–399. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otake, T., Hatano, G., Cutler, A., & Mehler, J. (1993). Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(2), 258–278. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oushiro, L. (2018). Transcritor fonológico do português [Portuguese phonological transcription tool]. Online version (v0.5.1). https://oushiro.shinyapps.io/silac. Access on April 2019.

  • Perfors, A. (2004). What’s in a name? The effect of sound symbolism on perception of facial attractiveness. In Proceedings of the annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 26(26). Retrieved from https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9bq5v5c7.

  • Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G. (2018). People can create iconic vocalizations to communicate various meanings to naïve listeners. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-20961-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia—a window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saji, N., Akita, K., Imai, M., Kantartzis, K., & Kita, S. (2013). Cross-linguistically shared and language-specific sound symbolism for motion: An exploratory data mining approach. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 35, 1253–1259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 225–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shih, S. S., Ackerman, J., Hermalin, N., Inkelas, S., & Kavitskaya, D. (2018). Pokémonikers: A study of sound symbolism and Pokémon names. Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America, 3(1), 42-1–6. https://doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v3i1.4335.

  • Shinohara, K., & Kawahara, S. (2010). A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: The images of size. Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 36(1), 396–410. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v36i1.3926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starr, R. L., Yu, A. C. L., & Shih, S. S. (2018). Sound symbolic effects in Mandarin and Cantonese personal names and Pokémon names. Talk at the first conference in Pokémonastics, May 2018, Keyo University: Japan.

  • Styles, S. J., & Gawne, L. (2017). When does Maluma/Takete fail? Two key failures and a meta-analysis suggest that phonology and phonotactics matter. I-Perception, 8(4), 2041669517724807. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669517724807.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tamaoka, K., & Makioka, S. (2004). Frequency of occurrence for units of phonemes, morae, and syllables appearing in a lexical corpus of a Japanese newspaper. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 531–547. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, C. Y., Nygaard, L. C., & Namy, L. L. (2017). The specificity of sound symbolic correspondences in spoken language. Cognitive Science, 41(8), 2191–2220. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viaro, M. E., & Guimarães-Filho, Z. O. (2007). Análise quantitativa da frequência dos fonemas e estruturas silábicas portuguesas [Frequency quantitative analysis of phonemes and syllabic structures in Portuguese]. Estudos Linguísticos, 36(1), 27–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westbury, C. (2005). Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence from an interference task. Brain and Language, 93(1), 10–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.07.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiseman, M., & van Peer, W. (2003). Roman Jakobsons Konzept der Selbstreferenz aus der Perspektive der heutigen Kognitionswissenschaft [Roman Jakobson’s concept of self-reference from the perspective of present-day cognition studies]. In H. Birus, S. Donat, & B. Meyer-Sickendiek (Eds.), Roman Jakobsons Gedichtanalysen. Eine Herausforderung an die Philologien (pp. 277–306). Göttingen: Wallstein.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. K., Hay, J., & Bent, T. (2005). Ladies first? Phonology, frequency, and the naming conspiracy. Linguistics, 43(3), 531–561. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.2005.43.3.531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Matheus Mafra for his work on the first and second experiments described in this paper. We are also grateful to Ms. toto-mame for allowing us to use her pictures for the current experiments. This project is partially supported by the JSPS grant #17K13448.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mahayana C. Godoy.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix A: Names for Experiment 2

Condition 1

Names expected for pre-evolution Pokémons

Names expected for post-evolution Pokémons

Pair 1

Rituto

Talusso

Pair 2

Rissuke

Namusse

Pair 3

Ticero

Natecho

Pair 4

Sifupe

Kacupe

Pair 5

Mirtelo

Nanseno

Pair 6

Sikelo

Katerro

Pair 7

Picro

Faplo

Pair 8

Rinco

Sampo

Pair 9

Nito

Lapo

Pair 10

Tinclo

Fantro

Condition 2

Names expected for pre-evolution Pokémons

Names expected for post-evolution Pokémons

Pair 1

Tinke

Pincepe

Pair 2

Pumo

Ruchelo

Pair 3

Paina

Laipefa

Pair 4

Nutax

Tuncecax

Pair 5

Rimpo

Nilnurro

Pair 6

Kampa

Kalsena

Pair 7

Mape

Latuste

Pair 8

Upex

Ulepex

Pair 9

Oma

Onura

Pair 10

Nifom

Rirechom

Condition 3

Names expected for pre-evolution Pokémons

Names expected for post-evolution Pokémons

Pair 1

Rilata

Dilapa

Pair 2

Miupessa

Ziutefa

Pair 3

Ruakel

Juachel

Pair 4

Lilnonox

Giscomox

Pair 5

Kofole

Gotoke

Pair 6

Crepifo

Dressimo

Pair 7

Fessuta

Denuna

Pair 8

Foama

Zoana

Pair 9

Sossepra

Domecra

Pair 10

Lompoco

Gostoro

Condition 4

Names expected for pre-evolution Pokémons

Names expected for post-evolution Pokémons

Pair 1

Pumpocla

Durzocla

Pair 2

Fripoma

Dribopa

Pair 3

Noporam

Bovomam

Pair 4

Secara

Vejacha

Pair 5

Namila

Babirra

Pair 6

Cenice

Bessize

Pair 7

Xitefar

Gimevar

Pair 8

Trapena

Graceba

Pair 9

Furofo

Dupojo

Pair 10

Flofesse

Glossebe

Appendix B: Names for Experiment 3

Condition 1 × 0

Names expected for pre-evolution Pokémons

Names expected for post-evolution Pokémons

Pair 1

Rilata

Dilapa

Pair 2

Miupessa

Ziutefa

Pair 3

Ruakel

Juachel

Pair 4

Lilnonox

Giscomox

Pair 5

Kofole

Gotoke

Pair 6

Crepifo

Dressimo

Pair 7

Fessuta

Denuna

Pair 8

Foama

Zoana

Pair 9

Sossepra

Domecra

Pair 10

Lompoco

Gostoro

Condition 2 × 0

Names expected for pre-evolution Pokémons

Names expected for post-evolution Pokémons

Pair 1

Pumpocla

Durzocla

Pair 2

Fripoma

Dribopa

Pair 3

Noporam

Bovomam

Pair 4

Secara

Vejacha

Pair 5

Namila

Babirra

Pair 6

Cenice

Bessize

Pair 7

Xitefar

Gimevar

Pair 8

Trapena

Graceba

Pair 9

Furofo

Dupojo

Pair 10

Flofesse

Glossebe

Condition 3 × 0

Names expected for pre-evolution Pokémons

Names expected for post-evolution Pokémons

Pair 1

Potula

Boduza

Pair 2

Rapomo

Zazogo

Pair 3

Funcrema

Zungreva

Pair 4

Cunetri

Dujedri

Pair 5

Mikepe

Zibeve

Pair 6

Flukini

Blubiji

Pair 7

Serselo

Jervego

Pair 8

Milete

Vijebe

Pair 9

Nipriro

Didrivo

Pair 10

Rofato

Vogazo

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Godoy, M.C., de Souza Filho, N.S., de Souza, J.G.M. et al. Gotta Name’em All: an Experimental Study on the Sound Symbolism of Pokémon Names in Brazilian Portuguese. J Psycholinguist Res 49, 717–740 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09679-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09679-2

Keywords

Navigation