Skip to main content
Log in

Does Self-Control Influence Maternal Attachment? A Reciprocal Effects Analysis from Early Childhood Through Middle Adolescence

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Quantitative Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

The purpose of this study is twofold. First, this study assesses the extent to which self-control and maternal attachment mutually influence one another. Second, it investigates whether this process continues to occur during adolescence. To date, studies of the etiology of self-control have yet to adequately address these issues, despite the fact that a number of theoretical perspectives emphasize the reciprocal nature of the parent-child relationship.

Methods

The current study seeks to shed light on these issues by examining the relationship between self-control and maternal attachment using structural equation modeling for eight waves of data spanning a period of time that encompasses early childhood through middle adolescence.

Results

The results yield two findings bearing on the adequacy of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s model of self-control development. First, measures of self-control and maternal attachment were found to mutually influence one another during childhood. Second, these effects were reduced to nonsignificance during adolescence.

Conclusions

This study finds that self-control emerges during childhood in a complex manner in which it both shapes and is shaped by parental attachment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The conditioning assured representation (at least 10 % marginally) of single parent households, mothers with less than a high school education, and ethnic minority mothers.

  2. There were 12 separate assessments, but the information collected at each assessment was not consistent in most cases. This, in part, guided the research design of the current study and limited our focus to the relationship between maternal attachment and self-control and not a broader array of parenting measures.

  3. Given that recruitment and enrollment of study families spanned from January, 1991 to November, 1991 there were slight variations in age across study children at each assessment.

  4. An additional consideration worth noting is that attitudinal, self-reported indicators of self-control may be highly unreliable at young ages. Thus, information from a parent is a practical necessity.

  5. The individual wave alpha values for self-control and maternal attachment are reported in Appendix.

  6. We did estimate second-order latent factor models using measures of parental monitoring and hostility, in addition to attachment, that were available at waves 7 and 8. This analysis indicated significant overlap exists between the indicators of attachment and the other dimensions of parenting, demonstrating that attachment is a valid indicator of parenting. Moreover, when we restricted model estimation to these two waves alone as opposed to all eight waves we found results that were substantively identical to those presented here.

  7. Differences in the N sizes in Table 1 for the race and sex variables relative to the items used to measure household type, self-control, and attachment should be explained. The information on child race and sex was obtained at the outset of the SECCYD and therefore no data is missing on these two variables. However, from the outset of the SECCYD to the assessment at age 4 approximately 20 % of the original study families dropped out of the study. To consider whether sample attrition was selective, we examined the sample’s composition with respect to five demographic/social status variables—sex, race, mother’s education, family structure, and family income—for which data were collected during the first interview when the study children were 1 month old. We uncovered no evidence of selective sample attrition.

  8. The serial correlations shown in Fig. 1 are not presented due to space constraints. The correlations varied from a minimum of 0.07 to a maximum of 0.4. No discernable pattern was apparent in the estimates, although constraining them to be zero substantially reduces the fit of the model.

  9. The reader may notice that some of the factor loadings are less than the traditional 0.7 rule of thumb. We tested whether separating items with weaker loadings into different latent factors provided a better fit to the model; but, these tests did not provide any significant evidence to support this approach.

  10. In fact, a model where the variance in self-control is constrained to be equal across the waves provides a better fit to the data (x 2(7) change = 11).

  11. This model can be identified using an instrumental variable approach. However, we are unable to locate a variable in our data set for which there is a correlation with either self-control or maternal attachment, but not the other construct. As a result, we must impose restrictions on the model for identification.

  12. Although a discussion of relative standardized effect sizes is informative, we should note that there is a known bias with the standardized effects of self-control on maternal attachment—because the standardized coefficients come from weighting the unstandardized coefficients by the ratio of the variances, the standardized coefficients for self-control predicting maternal attachment will get smaller due to the fact that the variances for maternal attachment increase over time. As such, the effect size of self-control on attachment in later waves of data are biased toward zero.

  13. We also examined these relationships using a second-order, dual-process latent growth curve model. If there is variation between individuals with respect to the intercept and slope for maternal attachment and self-control, then these properties will not be correctly examined with the current approach since the structural equation model examines the covariance matrix. We do not find any evidence of a substantial variance for the intercept or slope for either of these factors.

  14. Note that we are not testing the constraint that the effect of self-control at t on maternal attachment at t + 1 is equal to the effect of maternal attachment at t on self-control at t + 1 (i.e. cross-factor equality). Since the metrics for the items are not identical we cannot compare the unstandardized coefficients. Furthermore, since the variance for maternal attachment increases over time, the standardized coefficient for maternal attachment on self-control will be biased downward.

  15. Estimates for the effects of gender, race, and household type on self-control and maternal attachment showed little consistent effects on self-control or maternal attachment and thus were excluded from Table 4. In addition, we estimated the contemporaneous correlation between self-control and parental attachment. The correlation between the two latent factors is 0.2, 0.4 and 0.3 for waves 2, 3, and 4 respectively. These correlations represent the contemporaneous correlation between self-control and parental attachment after accounting for the cross-lagged and stability effects. Consistent with the estimates for the lagged effects, the contemporaneous correlation between the two latent factors for waves 5 through 8 are all less than 0.1 and are not significantly different from zero.

References

  • Akers RL (1991) Self-control as a general theory of crime. J Quant Criminol 7:201–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akers RL (2008) Self-control and social learning theory. In: Goode E (ed) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, California

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Wright JP (2007) A child effects explanation for the association between family risk and involvement in an antisocial lifestyle. J Adolesc Res 22:640–664

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell RQ (1979) Parent, child, and reciprocal influences. Am Psychol 34:821–826

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell RQ (1980) Socialization findings examined. In: Bell RQ, Harper LB (eds) Child effects on adults. University of Nebraska Press, Nebraska

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler PM (1985) Theory and implementation of EQS: a structural equations program. BMDP Statistical Software, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler PM (1990) Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychol Bull 107:238–246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler PM, Bonett DG (1980) Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol Bull 88:588–606

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler PM, Yuan KH (1999) Structural equation modeling with small samples: test statistics. Multivar Behav Res 34:181–197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt CH, Simons RL, Simons LG (2006) A longitudinal test of the effects of parenting and the stability of self-control: negative evidence for the general theory of crime. Criminology 44:353–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cernkovich SA, Giordano PC (1987) Family relationships and delinquency. Criminology 25:295–319

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapple CL (2005) Self-control, peer relations, and delinquency. Justice Q 22:89–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chou CP, Bentler PM, Satorra A (1991) Scaled test statistics and robust standard errors for nonnormal data in covariance structure analysis: a Monte Carlo study. Br J Math Stat Psychol 44:347–357

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curran PJ, West SG, Finch JF (1996) The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychol Methods 1:16–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dearing E, McCartney K, Taylor BA (2006) Within-child associations between family income and externalizing and internalizing problems. Dev Psychol 42:237–252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dempster AP, Laird NM, Rubin DB (1977) Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. J R Stat Soc 39:1–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman S, Weinberger DA (1994) Self-restraint as mediator of family influences on boys’ delinquent behavior: a longitudinal study. Child Dev 65:195–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gault-Sherman M (2012) It’s a two-way street: the bidirectional relationship between parenting and delinquency. J Youth Adolesc 41:121–145

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs JJ, Geiver D, Martin JS (1998) Parental management and self-control: an empirical test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory. J Res Crime Delinq 35:40–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goode E (2008) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, California

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T (1990) A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, California

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham JW (2009) Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annu Rev Psychol 60:549–576

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay C (2001) Parenting, low self-control, and delinquency: a test of self-control theory. Criminology 39:707–736

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay C, Forrest W (2006) The development of self-control: examining self-control theory’s stability thesis. Criminology 44:739–774

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay C, Forrest W (2008) Self-control and the concept of opportunity: the case for a more systematic union. Criminology 46:1039–1072

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi T (1969) Causes of delinquency. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi T, Gottfredson MR (2003) Punishment of children from the perspective of control theory. In: Britt CL, Gottfredson MR (eds) Advances in criminological theory, vol 12. Transaction Publishers, New Brunswick

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff P (2009) A first course in Bayesian statistical methods. Springer Science, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Holden GW (1997) Parents and the dynamics of child rearing. Westview Press, Boulder

    Google Scholar 

  • Hope TL, Grasmick HG, Pointon LJ (2003) The family in Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: structure, parenting, and self-control. Sociol Focus 36:291–311

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu L, Bentler PM, Kano Y (1992) Can test statistics in covariance structure analysis be trusted? Psychol Bull 112:351–362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamshidian M, Bentler PM (1999) ML estimation of mean and covariance structures with missing data using complete data routines. J Educ Behav Stat 24:21–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandel DB, Wu P (1995) Disentangling mother-child effects in the development of antisocial behavior. In: McCord J (ed) Coercion and punishment in long-term perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 106–123

    Google Scholar 

  • Kent D, Pepler D (2003) The aggressive child as agent in coercive family processes. In: Kuczynski L (ed) Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations. Sage, Thousand Oaks, pp 131–144

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr M, Stattin H (2000) What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment: further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Dev Psychol 36:366–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laird RD, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA (2003) Parents’ monitoring-relevant knowledge and adolescents’ delinquent behavior: evidence of correlated developmental changes and reciprocal influences. Child Dev 74:752–768

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loeber R, Loeber MS (1986) Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. Crime Justice 7:29–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsueda R, Anderson K (1998) The dynamics of delinquent peers and delinquent behavior. Criminology 36:269–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mcleod JD, Kruttschitt C, Dornfeld M (1994) Does parenting explain the effects of conditions on children’s antisocial behavior? A comparison of blacks and whites. Soc Forces 73:575–604

    Google Scholar 

  • Meldrum RC (2008) Beyond parenting: an examination of the etiology of self-control. J Crim Justice 36:244–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meldrum RC, Young JTN, Weerman FM (2009) Reconsidering the effect of self-control and delinquent peers: implications of measurement for theoretical significance. J Res Crime Delinq 46:353–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miner JL, Clarke-Stewart KA (2008) Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Dev Psychol 44:771–786

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt TE (1993) Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev 100:674–701

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network (2001) Nonmaternal care and family factors in early development: an overview of the NICHD study of early child care. Appl Dev Psychol 22:457–492

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nofziger S (2008) The “cause” of low self-control: the influence of maternal self-control. J Res Crime Delinq 45:191–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pardini DA, Fite PJ, Burke JD (2008) Bidirctional associations between parenting practices and conduct problems in boys from childhood to adolescence: the moderating role of age and African-American ethnicity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36:647–662

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson GR (1982) Coercive family process. Castilia, Eugene

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrone D, Sullivan CJ, Pratt TC, Margaryan S (2004) Parental efficacy, self-control, and delinquency: a test of a general theory of crime on a nationally representative sample of youth. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol 48:298–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peugh JL, Enders CK (2004) Missing data in educational research: a review of reporting practices and suggestions for improvement. Rev Educ Res 74:525–556

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phythian K, Keane C, Krull C (2008) Family structure and parental behavior: identifying the sources of adolescent self-control. West Criminol Rev 9:73–87

    Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR (2008) Measuring self-control. In: Goode E (ed) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Piquero AR, MacIntosh R, Hickman M (2000) Does self-control affect survey response? Applying exploratory, confirmatory, and item response theory analysis to Grasmick et al.’s self-control scale. Criminology 38:897–930

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Cullen FT (2000) The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: a meta-analysis. Criminology 38:931–964

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Turner MG, Piquero AR (2004) Parental socialization and community context: a longitudinal analysis of the structural sources of low self-control. J Res Crime Delinq 41:219–243

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sampson RJ, Laub JH (1993) Crime in the making: pathways and turning points through life. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Satorra A, Bentler PM (1994) Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In: Eye AV, Clogg CC (eds) Latent variables analysis: applications for developmental research. Sage, Thousand Oaks, pp 399–419

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder J, Cramer A, Afrank J, Patterson GR (2005) The contributions of ineffective discipline and parental hostile attributions of child misbehavior to the development of conduct problems at home and school. Dev Psychol 41:30–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steiger JH (1990) Structural model evaluation and modification: an internal estimation approach. Multivar Behav Res 25:173–180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaxton S, Agnew R (2004) The nonlinear effects of parent and teacher attachment on delinquency: disentangling strain from social control explanations. Justice Q 21:763–791

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornberry TP (1987) Toward an interactional theory of delinquency. Criminology 25:863–891

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornberry TP, Lizotte AJ, Krohn MD, Farnsworth M, Jang SJ (1991) Testing interactional theory: an examination of reciprocal causal relationships among family, school, and delinquency. J Crim Law Criminol 82:3–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tittle CR, Ward DA, Grasmick HG (2003) Self-control and crime/deviance: cognitive vs. behavioral measures. J Quant Criminol 19:333–366

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay RE (1995) Kindergarten behavioral patterns, parental practices, and early adolescent antisocial behavior. In: McCord J (ed) Coercion and punishment in long-term perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 139–153

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner MG, Piquero AR (2002) The stability of self-control. J Crim Justice 30:457–471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unnever JD, Cullen FT, Pratt TC (2003) Parental management, ADHD, and delinquent involvement: reassessing Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory. Justice Q 20:471–500

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Voorhis P, Cullen FT, Mathers RA, Garner CC (1988) The impact of family structure and quality on delinquency: a comparative assessment of structural and functional factors. Criminology 26:235–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vazsonyi AT, Belliston LM (2007) The family → low self-control → deviance. Crim Justice Behav 34:505–530

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vazsonyi AT, Pickering LE, Junger M, Hessing D (2001) An empirical test of a general theory of crime: a four-nation comparative study of self-control and the prediction of deviance. J Res Crime Delinq 38:91–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warr M (2007) The tangled web: delinquency, deception, and parental attachment. J Youth Adolesc 36:607–622

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright JP, Beaver KM (2005) Do parents matter in creating self-control in their children? A genetically informed test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s theory of low self-control. Criminology 43:1169–1202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright JP, Cullen FT (2001) Parental efficacy and delinquent behavior: do control and support matter? Criminology 39:677–705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan KH, Bentler PM (1998) Normal theory based test statistics in structural equation modeling. Br J Math Stat Psychol 51:289–309

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD) was conducted by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Early Child Care Research Network, supported by NICHD through a cooperative agreement that calls for scientific collaboration between the grantees and the NICHD staff.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ryan C. Meldrum.

Additional information

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development or the National Institutes of Health (United States Department of Health and Human Services. National Institutes of Health. Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development: Phases I–IV, 1991–2008 [United States] [Computer files]. ICPSR21940-v1; ICPSR21941-v1; ICPSR21942-v1; ICPSR22361-v1. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor]).

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 5.

Table 5 Robust maximum likelihood estimates for measurement model of self-control and maternal attachment (n = 1,364)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meldrum, R.C., Young, J.T.N., Hay, C. et al. Does Self-Control Influence Maternal Attachment? A Reciprocal Effects Analysis from Early Childhood Through Middle Adolescence. J Quant Criminol 28, 673–699 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-012-9173-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-012-9173-y

Keywords

Navigation