Skip to main content
Log in

Children’s Susceptibility to Misidentifying a Familiar Bystander From a Lineup: When Younger is Better

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

Children from 5 to 12 years of age (N=779) were shown a videotape where a preschool teacher has money stolen from her wallet. Children were shown a lineup, and for children in the bystander condition, the lineup contained a familiar bystander without the thief. Children in the control condition viewed the same lineup but they had not seen the bystander in the videotape. Among the 11–12-year olds, participants in the bystander condition were significantly more likely than control participants to misidentify the familiar bystander. This effect was not found in children from 5 to 10 years of age. When children in the control condition were shown a lineup that contained the thief without the bystander, the 11–12-year olds were significantly more likely than the younger children to correctly identify the thief. These findings demonstrate that age can both increase and decrease the accuracy of children’s lineup identification accuracy depending on the task at hand and the content of a lineup.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, B. P. (1984). Storage and processing constraints on integrating story information in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 64–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman, B. P. (1992). The sources of children’s source errors in judging causal inferences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 90–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beal, C. R., Schmitt, K. L., & Dekle, D. J. (1995). Eyewitness identification of children. Law and Human Behavior, 19, 197–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F. & Forrest, T. J. (2002). Are young children susceptible to the False-Memory Illusion? Child Development, 73, 1363–1377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, E., Deffenbacher, K., & Sturgill, W. (1977). Memory for faces and the circumstances of encounter. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 311–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckhout, R. (1974). Eyewitness testimony. Scientific American, 231, 23–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. W., Wellman, H., Keniston, A. H., & Westhy, S. D. (1978). Age-related aspects of comprehension and inference from a televised dramatic narrative. Child Development, 49, 389–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dekle, D. J., Beal, C. R., Elliott, R., & Huneycutt, D. (1996). Children as witnesses: A comparison of lineup versus showup identification methods. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dysart, J. E., Lindsay, R. C. L., Hammond, R. & Dupuis, P. (2001). Mug shot exposure prior to lineup identification: Interference, transference, and commitment effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 1280–1284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geiselman, E. R., Haghighi, D., & Stown, R. (1996). Unconscious transference and characteristics of accurate and inaccurate eyewitnesses. Psychology, Crime and Law, 2, 197–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geiselman, E. R., MacArthur, A., & Meerovitch, S. (1993). Transference of perpetrator roles in eyewitness identifications from photoarrays. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 11, 5–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, G., & Reed, R. S. (1986). Age differences in eyewitness testimony. Law and Human Behavior, 10, 317–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorenstein, G. W., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1980). Effect of choosing an incorrect photograph on a later identification by an eyewitness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 616–622.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (1996). Eyewitness identifications by 5- to 6-year-old children. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 359–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houts, M. (1956). From evidence to guilt. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, H. & Smith, L. B. (1981). Children’s inferential abilities in the context of reading to understand. Child Development, 52, 1216–1223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C. L., Pozzulo, J. D., & Craig, W., Lee, K., & Corber, S. (1997). Simultaneous lineups, sequential lineups, and showups: Eyewitness identification decisions of adults and children. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 391–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F. (1976). Unconscious transference. Law and Psychology Review, 2, 93–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G., & Lindauer, B. K. (1976). The role of inference in children’s comprehension and memory for sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 217–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G., Lindauer, B. K. & Cox, G. L. (1977). The development of inferential comprehension. Child Development, 48, 1728–1733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, J. F. (1995). Age differences in source monitoring of performed and imagined actions on immediate and delayed tests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 84–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, J. F., & Carranza, L. E. (1989). Eyewitness testimony of children in target present and target absent lineups. Law and Human Behavior, 13, 133–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, J. F., & Myers, A. (2001). Attempts to improve children’s identifications from sequential lineups. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 796–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, J. F., & Ryan, V. (1993). An attempt to reduce guessing behavior in children’s and adults’ eyewitness identifications. Law and Human Behavior, 17, 11–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfect, T. J. & Harris, L. J. (2003). Adult age differences in unconscious transference: Source confusion or identity blending? Memory and Cognition, 31, 570–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pezdek, K. (1980). Life-span differences in semantic integration of pictures and sentences in memory. Child Development, 51, 720–729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, M., Geiselman, R., Haghighi, D., & Lin, C. (1997). Some boundary conditions for bystander misidentification. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 24, 370–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pozzulo, J. D. & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1997). Increasing correct identifications by children. Expert Evidence, 5, 126–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pozzulo, J. D. & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1998). Identification accuracy of children versus adults: A meta-analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 549–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pozzulo, J. D. & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1999). Elimination lineups: An improved identification procedure for child eyewitnesses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 167–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read, J. D., Tollestrup, P., Hammersley, R., McFadzen, E., & Christensen, A. (1990). The unconscious transference effect: Are innocent bystanders ever misidentified? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 3–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, D. F., Ceci, S. J., Dunning, D., & Toglia, M. P. (1994). Unconscious transference and mistaken identity: When a witness misidentifies a familiar but innocent person. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 918–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David F. Ross.

About this article

Cite this article

Ross, D.F., Marsil, D.F., Benton, T.R. et al. Children’s Susceptibility to Misidentifying a Familiar Bystander From a Lineup: When Younger is Better. Law Hum Behav 30, 249–257 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9034-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9034-z

Keywords

Navigation