Skip to main content
Log in

Consumer Choices Under Small Probabilities: Overweighting or Underweighting?

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article provides a theoretical account and identifies boundary conditions for the common beliefs about the “overweighting of small probabilities”. Based on four studies, our results suggest that the weighting of probabilities is context-dependent. When the contrast in value is reduced or not presented in a decision problem, small probabilities are less likely to be overweighted. In addition, as the associated payoffs increase, the weights on small probabilities tend to diminish or even to be “underweighted”. However, there is an “interpersonal difference” in the weighting of probabilities which reflects the particular circumstances of individuals, including their wealth status. Some implications of small probabilities for marketing practices are also investigated and discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abdellaoui, Mohammed. (2000). “Parameter-Free Elicitation of Utility and Probability Weighting Functions,” Management Science 46(Nov.), 1497–1512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bleichrodt, Han and Jose Luis Pinto. (2000). “A Parameter–Free Elicitation of the Probability Weighting Function in Medical Decision Analysis,” Management Science 46(Nov.), 1485–1496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choong, Peggy. (2001). “Preventing or Fixing a Problem: a Marketing Manager’s Dilemma Revisited,” The Journal of Services Marketing, 15(2), 99–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, Robert J. and Geoffrey Jalleh. (1999). “Positively versus Negatively Framed Product Attributes: The Influence of Involvement,” Psychology & Marketing 16(Oct.), 613–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J. M. and J. G. Lynch, Jr. (1988). “Self-Generated Validity and Other Effects of Measurement on Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior,” Journal of Applied Psychology 73(Aug.), 421–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, Gregory W., Ziv Carmon, Dan Ariely, and Gal Zauberman. (1999). “Goal-based Construction of Preferences: Task Goals and the Prominence Effect,” Management Science 45(Aug.), 1057–1075.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, Richard and George Wu. (1999). “On the Shape of the Probability Weighting Function,” Cognitive Psychology 38(Feb.), 129–166.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth, R. M. and H. J. Einhorn. (1990). “Venture Theory: A Model of Decision Weights,” Management Science 36(July), 780–803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, Christopher K., Sally Blount, George F. Loewenstein, and Maz H. Maz. (1999). “Preference Reversals between Joint and Separate Evaluations of Options: A Review and Theoretical Analysis,” Psychological Bulletin 125(Sept.), 576–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janiszewski, Chris and Luk Warlop. (1993). “The Influence of Classical Conditioning Procedures on Subsequent Attention to the Conditioned Brand,” Journal of Consumer Research 20(Sept.), 171–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Amos Tversky. (1979). “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk,” Econometrica 47(March), 263–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Amos Tversky. (1984). “Choices, Values, and Frames,” American Psychologist 39(4), 341–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel, J. L. Knetsch, and R. H. Thaler. (1991). “The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion and Status Quo Bias,” Journal of Economic Perspectives 17(Winter), 193–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kilka, M. and M. Weber. (2001). “What Determines the Shape of the Probability Weighting Function under Uncertainty?” Management Science 47(Dec.), 1712–1726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, Kris N. and Mariana Santiesteban. (2003). “Concave Utility, Transaction Costs, and Risk in Measuring Discounting of Delayed Rewards,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 29(Jan.), 66–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, Irwin P., Judy Schreiber, Marco Lauriola, and Gary J. Gaeth. (2002). “A Tale of Two Pizzas: Building Up from a Basic Product Versus Scaling Down from a Fully-Loaded Product,” Marketing Letters 13(Nov.), 335–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J. G., Jr., H. Marmorstein, and M. F. Weigold. (1988). “Choices from Sets Including Remembered Brands: Use of Recalled Attributes and Prior Overall Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Research 15(Sept.), 169–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazumdar, Tridib and Purushottam Papatla. (2000). “An Investigation of Reference Price Segments,” Journal of Marketing Research 37(May), 246–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCool, Steven F. (1991). “Using Probabilistic Incentives to Increase Response Rates to Mail-Return Highway Intercept Diaries,” Journal of Travel Research 30(Fall), 17–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowlis, Stephen M. and Itamar Simonson. (1997). “Attribute-Task Compatibility as Determinant of Consumer Preference Reversals,” Journal of Marketing Research 34(May), 205–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prelec, D. (1998). “The Probability Weighting Function,” Econometrica 66(May), 497–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preston, M. and P. Baratta. (1948). “An Experimental Study of the Auction-Value of an Uncertain Outcome,” American Journal of Psychology 61, 183–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rajendran, K. N. and Gerard J. Tellis (1994), “Contextual and Temporal Components of Reference Price,” Journal of Marketing 58(Jan.), 22–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, Paul S., Alan S. Dick, and Arun K. Jain. (1994). “Extrinsic and Intrinsic Cue Effects on Perceptions of Store Brand Quality,” Journal of Marketing 58(Oct.), 28–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rottenstreich, Yuval and Christopher K. Hsee. (2001). “Money, Kisses, and Electric Shocks: On the Affective Psychology of Risk,” Psychological Science 12(May), 185–190.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, William and Richard Zeckhauser. (1988). “Status Quo Bias in Decision Making,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 1(March), 7–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schkade, David A. and Eric J. Johnson. (1989). “Cognitive Processes in Preference Reversals,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 44(Oct.), 203–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, Richard. (1985). “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,” Marketing Science 4(Summer), 199–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. (1991). “Loss Aversion in Riskless Choice: A Reference-Dependent Model,” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 106(Nov.), 1039–1061.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. (1992). “Advanced in Prospect Theory: Cumulative Representations of Uncertainty,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 5, 297–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos, S. Sattath, and P. Slovic. (1988). “Contingent Weighting in Judgment and Choice,” Psychological Review 95(3), 371–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, George and R. Gonzalez. (1999). “Nonlinear Decision Weights in Choice under Uncertainty,” Management Science 45(Jan.), 74–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, George. (1999). “Anxiety and Decision Making with Delayed Resolution of Uncertainty,” Theory and Decision 46(April), 159–198.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rong Chen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chen, R., Jia, J. Consumer Choices Under Small Probabilities: Overweighting or Underweighting?. Market Lett 16, 5–18 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-005-1137-7

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-005-1137-7

Keywords

Navigation