Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of cognitive resource availability on consumer decisions involving counterfeit products: The role of perceived justification

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In four experiments, participants made a purchase decision about a counterfeit product under either constrained or unconstrained cognitive resource conditions. Participants were less likely to purchase the counterfeit when their cognitive resources were constrained than when they were not. However, this difference was evident only when individuals had strong (vs. weak) moral beliefs, or when they had low (vs. high) accountability for their decisions. These and other results suggest that the effect of cognitive resource availability on counterfeit purchase was mediated by participants’ perceptions of justification about the purchase.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Counterfeit purchases can occur in two different ways. First, consumers may be deceived to believe that the counterfeit products they purchase are genuine ones. This is termed a ‘deceptive’ counterfeiting (Grossman and Shapiro 1998). Second, consumers may purchase counterfeits even if they know that the products are counterfeits. This is termed a “non-deceptive” counterfeiting (Grossman and Shapiro 1998). The focus of the present research is the latter.

  2. Note that justification questions were asked always after participants indicated their purchase intentions. To this extent, participants’ justifications might have been answered to be consistent with their purchase intentions and did not actually drive the decision of purchasing the counterfeit. To evaluate this possibility, we performed a reverse mediation analysis in which justification rating was a dependent variable while purchase intention served as a mediator. This analysis indicated that including purchase intentions as additional predictor in the model did not eliminate the effect of resource availability on justification ratings to nonsignificant (β = 0.85, t = 1.65, p < 0.10), thus calling the validity of the alternative possibility into questions.

  3. A reverse mediation analysis indicated that including purchase intention as additional predictor in the regression model did not eliminate the effect of resource availability on justification rations to nonsignificance (β = 0.57; t = 1.77; p < 0.10), as consistent with results in experiment 1.

References

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cordell, V. V., Wongtada, N., & Kieschnick, R. L., Jr. (1996). Counterfeit purchase intentions: Role of lawfulness attribute and product traits as determinants. Journal of Business Research, 35(1), 41–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cushman, F., Young, L., & Hauser, M. (2006). The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgments: Testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1082–1089.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Death to dichotomizing. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 5–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fullerton, R. A., & Punj, G. (2004). Repercussions of promoting an ideology of consumption: Consumer misbehavior. Journal of Business Research, 57(11), 1239–1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Norton, M. I., & Ariely, D. (2010). The counterfeit self: The deceptive costs of faking it. Psychological Science, 21(5), 712–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., & Paxton, J. M. (2009). Patterns of neural activity associated with honest and dishonest moral decisions. PNAS, 106(30), 12506–12511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Morelli, S. A., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107(1), 1144–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, G., & Shapiro, C. (1998). Foreign counterfeiting of status goods. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 103(412), 79–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttierez, C., Verheugen, G., Mandelson, P., & Schwab, S. (2006). Countering counterfeits. Wall Street Journal (June 20). A20

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 998–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, Y. J., Nunes, J. C., & Drèze, X. (2010). Signaling status with luxury goods: The role of brand prominence. Journal of Marketing Research, 74(4), 15–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • International Anticounterfeiting Coalition. About counterfeiting. Retrieved from http://iacc.org/about-counterfeiting. Accessed 7 June 2011

  • Kohlberg, L. (1984). The philosophy of moral development: Essays on moral development. San Francisco: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalwani, A. K. (2009). The distinct influence of cognitive busyness and need for closure on cultural differences in socially desirable responding. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(August), 305–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logsdon, J. M., Thompson, J. K., & Reid, R. A. (1994). Software piracy: I it related to level of moral judgment? Journal of Business Ethics, 13(11), 849–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazar, N., & Aggarwal, P. (2011). Greasing the palm: Can collectivism promote bribery? Psychological Science, 22(7), 843–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A self-concept maintenance theory. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moores, T. T., & Chang, J. C.-J. (2006). Ethical decision making in software piracy: Initial development and test of a four-component model. MIS Quarterly, 30(1), 167–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muncy, J., & Vitell, S. (1992). Consumer ethics: An investigation of the ethical beliefs of the final consumer. Journal of Business Research, 24(4), 297–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(March), 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M. R., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 774–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, M., & Hsee, C. K. (2002). Stretching the truth: Elastic justification and motivated communication of uncertain information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 25(2), 185–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(December), 278–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (2002). Spontaneous versus controlled influences of stimulus-based affect on choice behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 87(March), 342–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and complexity of thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(1), 74–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1986). A value pluralism model of ideological reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), 819–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(3), 379–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tom, G., Garibaldi, B., & Pilcher, J. (1998). Consumer demand for counterfeit goods. Psychology and Marketing, 15(5), 405–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science, 17(6), 476–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitell, S., & Muncy, J. (2005). The Muncy-Vitell consumer ethics scale: A modification and application. Journal of Business Ethics, 62(2), 267–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vohs, K., & Faber, R. (2007). Spent resources: Self-regulatory resource availability affects impulse buying. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(March), 537–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wee, C.-H., Tan, S.-J., & Cheok, K.-H. (1995). Non-price determinants of intention to purchase counterfeit goods. International Marketing Review, 12(6), 19–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilcox, K., Kim, H. M., & Sen, S. (2009). Why do consumers buy counterfeit luxury brands? Journal of Marketing Research, 46(2), 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Y., & Mittal, V. (2005). Decision difficulty: Effects of procedural and outcome accountability. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(December), 465–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the editor and the reviewers for their constructive comments on earlier drafts. This research was supported by the Hyundai-Kia Research Fellow Fund to Jongwon Park.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jungkeun Kim.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kim, J., Kim, JE. & Park, J. Effects of cognitive resource availability on consumer decisions involving counterfeit products: The role of perceived justification. Mark Lett 23, 869–881 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-012-9194-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-012-9194-1

Keywords

Navigation