Skip to main content
Log in

The emergence of emotions and religious sentiments during the September 11 disaster

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Analyzing emotional states under duress or during heightened, life-and-death situations is extremely difficult, especially given the inability of laboratory experiments to replicate the environment and given the inherent biases of post event surveys. This is where natural experiments, such as the pager communications from September 11th can provide the kind of natural experiment emotion researchers have been seeking. We demonstrate that positive and pro-social communications are the first to emerge followed by the slower and lower negative communications. Religious sentiment is the last to emerge, as individual attempt to make sense of event. Additionally we provide a methodological discussion about the preparation and analysis of such natural experiments (the pager message content) and show the importance of using multiple methods to extract the broadest possible understanding.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. While this research does show the change in emotion over time, the one week block sizes only provides a coarse analysis and does not specifically look at the event as it is occurring.

  2. Note, the BKE dataset only begins at 6:45 am.

  3. Communication Timeline: 8:14 am American Airlines Flight 11 hijacked, between 8:42–8:46 am United Airlines Flight 175 aircraft hijacked. 8:46:40 am American 11 flew into WTC Tower 1. 8:51–8:54 am American Airlines Flight 77 hijacked. 9:02:59 am United Airlines Flight 175 hits south side of WTC Tower 2. 9:37:46 am, American Airlines Flight 77 crashes into the Pentagon. 9:28 am hijackers take over United Airlines Flight 93. 9:58:59 am Collapse of WTC Tower 2. 10:02:23 am Flight 93 crashes into an empty field in Pennsylvania. 10:28:22 am Collapse of WTC 1.

  4. We believe that the results do indicate a positive swing towards religion and not merely the use of phrases such as “oh my god” after the event. When this phrase is analysed we find that it occurs only 14 times through out the day—12 of which occur between 8 am–11 am.

References

  • Andreoni, J., & Miller, J. H. (2002). Giving according to garp: An experimental test of the consistency of preferences for altruism. Econometrica, 70, 737–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22(1), 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aspinwall, L. G. (2004). Dealing with adversity: Self-regulation, coping, adaptation, and health. Applied Social Psychology, 1, 3–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Back, M. D., Küfner, A. C., & Egloff, B. (2010). The emotional timeline of September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1417–1419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Back, M. D., Küfner, A. C. P., & Egloff, B. (2011). Automatic or the people?. Psychological Science, 22(6), 837–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, D., Pate, S., Lawless, H., Sparkman, P., Lambers, S., & Worman, B. (1979). Helping under conditions of common threat: Increased we-feeling or ensuring reciprocity. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 410–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1974). A theory of social interaction. Journal of the Political Economy, 41(1), 54–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, G. E., & Ockenfels, A. (2000). Erc: Theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. American Economic Review, 90, 166–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F. (2004). Prospect theory in the wild: Evidence from the field. In C. F. Camerer, G. Loewenstein, & M. Rabin (Eds.), Advances in behavioral economics (pp. 148–161). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic markers of psychological change surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 15(10), 687–693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drago, R., & Garvey, G. T. (1998). Incentives for helping on the job: Theory and evidence. Journal of Labor Economics, 16, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dufwenberg, M., & Kirchsteiger, G. (2004). A theory of sequential reciprocity. Games and Economic Behavior, 47, 268–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. (2007). Explaining social behaviour. More nuts and bolts for the social sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A. (2005). Striving for the sacred: Personal goals, life meaning, and religion. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 731–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A., & Paloutzian, R. F. (2003). The psychology of religion. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 377–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114, 817–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. American Psychologist, 55(6), 647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foltz, P. W., Kintsch, W., & Landauer, T. K. (1998). The measurement of textual coherence with latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25(2&3), 285–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1901). Psychopathology of everyday life. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S. (1997). Not just for the money: An economic theory of personal motivation. London: Cheltenham.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., Savage, D. A., & Torgler, B. (2010). Interaction of natural survival instincts and internalized social norms exploring the Titanic and Lusitania disasters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(11), 4862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, R. (2001). Redeveloping diction: Theoretical considerations. In M. West (Ed.), Theory, method and practice in computer content analysis. New York: Alblex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heide, E. A. d. (2004). Common misconceptions about disasters: Panic, the disaster syndrome, and looting. In M. O’Leary (Ed.), The first 72 hours: A community approach to disaster preparedness (pp. 340–380). Lincoln: iUniverse Publishing.

  • Hogg, M. A., Adelman, J. R., & Blagg, R. D. (2010). Religion in the face of uncertainty: An uncertainty-identity theory account of religiousness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 72–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hood, R. W., Hill, P. C., & Spilka, B. (2009). The psychology of religion. An empirical approach. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hård Segerstad, Y. (2005). Language in sms a socio-linguistic view. In R. Harper et al. (Eds.), The inside text, volume 4 of the Kluwer international series on computer supported cooperative work (pp. 33–51). Netherlands: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kean, T. H., & Hamilton, L. (2004). 9/11 commission report: Final report of the national commission on terrorist attacks upon the United States. New York:WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. In E. Jones, D. Kanouse, H. Kelley, R. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacan, J. (1968). The language of the self: The function of language in psychoanalysis. Baltimore: John Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ling, R. (2003). The socio-linguistics of SMS: An analysis of SMS use by a random sample of Norwegians. Mobile Communication and the Renegotiation of the Social Sphere, 31(4), 335–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martos, T., Kézdy, A., & Horváth-Szabó, K. (2011). Religious motivations for everyday goals: Their religious context and potential consequences. Motivation and Emotion, 35(1), 75–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mawson, A. R. (2007). Mass panic and social attachment: The dynamics of human behavior. Ashgate: Aldershot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pargament, K. I., Magyar-Russell, G. M., & Murray-Swank, N. A. (2005). The sacred and the search for significance: Religion as a unique process. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 665–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, C. L. (2005). Religion as a meaning-making framework in coping with life stress. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 707–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547–577.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Popping, R. (2000). Computer-assisted text analysis. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pury, C. L. S. (2011). Automation can lead to confounds in text analysis: Back, Küfner, and Egloff (2010) and the not-so-angry Americans. Psychological Science, 22(6), 835–836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rabin, M. (1993). Incorperating fairness into game theory and economics. The Americian Economic Review, 83(5), 1281–1302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ripley, A. (2008). The unthinkable: Who survives when disaster strikes—and why?. London: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savage, D. A., & Torgler, B. (2010). Fairness and allocations systems. Economic Analysis and Policy, 40(2), 229–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberman, I. (2005). Religion as a meaning system: Implications for the new millennium. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 641–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, J. (2005). Interdependent preferences and reciprocity. Journal of Economic Literature, 42, 392–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spilka, B., Shaver, P., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1985). A general attribution theory for the psychology of religion. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 24(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • The Federal Building & Fire Saftey Investigation of the World Trade Center Disaster. (2008). Final report on the collapse of world trade center building 7 (Tech. Rep.). National institute of Standards and Technology: US Department of Commerce.

  • Thurlow, C. (2003). Generation txt? Exposing the sociolinguistics of young peopleŠs text messaging (Vol. 1) (No. 1). Sheffield Hallam University, 20th June 2012.

  • Tix, A. P., & Frazier, P. A. (2005). Mediation and moderation of the relationship between intrinsic religiousness and mental health. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(3), 295–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2007). Tax compliance and tax morale: A theoretical and empirical analysis. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, R. P. (1990). Basic content analysis (Vol. 7). Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David A. Savage.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Savage, D.A., Torgler, B. The emergence of emotions and religious sentiments during the September 11 disaster. Motiv Emot 37, 586–599 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9330-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9330-5

Keywords

Navigation