Skip to main content
Log in

How guilt leads to reparation? Exploring the processes underlying the effects of guilt

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is widely assumed that guilt leads people to engage into reparatory behaviors. However, the processes underlying this effect are in need for further specification. Four studies tested potential underlying cognitive mechanisms. Results suggest that guilt increases attention toward positive and reparation-oriented cues (Study 1) and makes attitudes toward reparation-oriented primes more positive (Study 3). No effect was found for accessibility of reparation words (Studies 2a, b). Taken together, these results suggest that guilt leads people to pay more attention to reparation means and to develop a more positive attitude toward reparation means, but does not render reparatory means more accessible. Implications for a better knowledge of guilt’s behavioral consequences are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For each study, we planned to run at least 20 participants in each condition (Simmons et al. 2011) , and enrolled as many participants as possible in the time devoted to the experimental sessions.

  2. No moderating effect of gender was found for any of the four studies, Fs < 1.23.

  3. Participants were asked what their mother tongue was and, if not French, at what age did learn French. Only French natives and participants who learnt French during early childhood (before 5 years old) were kept.

  4. Only inverse-transformed data were used for statistical calculation, including bias indexes. Hence confidence intervals for untransformed data are not relevant and not presented here.

  5. We should note that a third experiment was carried out on accessibility, using another progressive demasking paradigm (“PDM”, Feustel et al. 1983, see also; Ferrand et al. 2011). Like in Studies 2a and 2b, no interaction was found between emotion and word type, F = 0.56. It is also worth noticing that including this study in the small scale meta-analysis does not modify the results.

References

  • Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & de Vries, P. (2001). On the psychology of drinking: Being thirsty and perceptually ready. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 631–642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alexopoulos, T., Fiedler, K., & Freytag, P. (2012). The impact of open and closed mindsets on evaluative priming. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 978–994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexopoulos, T., Muller, D., Ric, F., & Marendaz, C. (2012). I, me, mine: Automatic attentional capture by self-related stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 770–779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asmundson, G. J. G., Carleton, R. N., & Ekong, J. (2005). Dot-probe evaluation of selective attentional processing of pain cues in patients with chronic headaches. Pain, 114(1–2), 250–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D. P. (1955). Relationships between shame and guilt in the socializing process. Psychological review, 62, 378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 361–382). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt as interpersonal phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W. (1966). Theory of psychological reactance. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlsmith, J. M., & Gross, A. E. (1969). Some effects of guilt on compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 232–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cotte, J., Coulter, R. A., & Moore, M. (2005). Enhancing or disrupting guilt: The role of ad credibility and perceived manipulative intent. Journal of Business Research, 58, 361–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coulter, R. H., Cotte, J., & Moore, M. L. (1997). Guilt appeals in advertising: Are you feeling guilty? In D. T. LeClairet & M. Hartline (Eds.), AMA winter educators’ conference: Marketing theory and applications (pp. 109–115). Chicago: American Marketing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cryder, E. C., Springer, S., & Morewedge, C. K. (2012). Guilty feelings, targeted actions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 607–618.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Hooge, I. E., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2008). Not so ugly after all: When shame acts as a commitment device. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Hooge, I. E., Nelissen, R.M.A., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2011). What is moral about guilt? Acting “prosocially” at the disadvantage of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 462–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1025–1042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Rucker, D. D., Wegener, D. T., & Braverman, J. (2004). Discrete emotions and persuasion: The role of emotion-induced expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 43–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, M. J. (2008). On becoming ready to pursue a goal you don’t know you have: Effects of nonconscious goals on evaluative readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1268–1294.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 557–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrand, L., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., New, B., Bonin, P., Meot, A., & Pallier, C. (2011). Comparing word processing times in naming, lexical decision, and progressive demasking: Evidence from chronolex. Frontiers in Psychology, 42, 488–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feustel, T. C., Shiffrin, R. M., & Salasoo, A. (1983). Episodic and lexical contributions to the repetition effect in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 309–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: An integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graton, A., Ric, F., & Gonzalez, E. (2016). Reparation or reactance? The influence of guilt on reaction to persuasive communication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 62, 40–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 505–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 429–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, C. W., & Cidam, A. (2015). When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. M., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manstead, A. S. R., Tetlock, P. E., & Manstead, T. (1989). Cognitive appraisals and emotional experience: Further evidence. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 225–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L. Jr., & Rachal, K. C. (1997). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 321–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Orienting of attention to threatening facial expressions presented under conditions of restricted awareness. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 713–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, S. G., Ferguson, M. J., & Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Affect in the aftermath: How goal pursuit influences implicit evaluations. Cognition & Emotion, 25, 453–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nabi, R. L. (2002). Discrete emotions and persuasion. In J. Dillard & M. Pfau (Eds.), Handbook of persuasion (pp. 289–308). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelissen, R. M. A., Dijker, A. J. M., & deVries, N. K. (2007). How to turn a hawk into a dove and vice versa: Interactions between emotions and goals in a give-some dilemma game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 280–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Innes-Ker, A. H. (1999). Emotional response categorization. Psychological Review, 106, 337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 510–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S., & Rubenstein, M. (1971). Evidence for phoneInic recoding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 10, 645–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2006). The approach and avoidance function of guilt and shame emotions: Comparing reactions to self-caused and other-caused wrongdoing. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 43–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological science, 22, 1359–1366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Geschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460–1469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuewig, J., Tangney, J. P., Heigel, C., & Harty, L. (2008). Shaming, blaming, and maiming: Links among the moral emotions and aggression. In J. Stuewig (Ed.), Inside the criminal mind: Findings from the GMU Inmate Study. Boston, MA: Symposium conducted at the annual convention of the American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, M., & Robinson, M. D. (2007). The happy spotlight: Positive mood and selective attention to rewarding information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1124–1136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. (2002). Shame and Guilt. New York: Guilford.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Miller, R., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1256–1269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 375–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, F. W., Payne, G. C., & Morgan, R. D. (1983). Participant descriptions of guilt and shame. Motivation and Emotion, 7, 25–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg, M., Nelissen, R. M., Breugelmans, S. M., & Pieters, R. (2008). On emotion specificity in decision making: Why feeling is for doing. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 18–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2006). Feeling is for doing: A pragmatic approach to the study of emotions in economic behavior. In D. DeCremer, M. Zeelenberg & J. K. Murnighan (Eds.), Social psychology and economics (pp. 117–137). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zillmann, D. (1988). Mood management: Using entertainment to full advantages. In L. Donohew & H. E. Sypher (Eds.), Communication, social cognition, and affect (pp. 147–171). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Aurélien Graton or François Ric.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Aurélien Graton declares that he has no conflict of interest. François Ric declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Graton, A., Ric, F. How guilt leads to reparation? Exploring the processes underlying the effects of guilt. Motiv Emot 41, 343–352 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9612-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9612-z

Keywords

Navigation