Skip to main content
Log in

Social Influences on Online Political Information Search and Evaluation

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Americans are turning to the Internet to learn about politics in greater and greater numbers. Under the current “Web 2.0” paradigm in which users are encouraged to interact with online content, voters encountering political information on the Internet are typically exposed to more than just the news; online information is often colored by the reactions of previous readers, whether in the form of displayed comments or in readily apparent tallies of the number of “likes” or “shares” a particular item has received. In this paper we consider the effect these social cues have on online political information search and evaluation. Using processing-tracing software to monitor the patterns of information search and evaluation among our subjects, we find that social cues can function as a heuristic, allowing voters to reach judgments similar to those of their more informed counterparts. However, we also find that negative cues can adversely influence candidate evaluation, making subjects less disposed to a candidate than they would be in the absence of such signals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Report available at http://www.pewinternet.org/2009/04/15/the-internets-role-in-campaign-2008/.

  2. Indeed, the social cues common to online media have begun to appear in more traditional outlets, as some news programs now incorporate viewer comments and reactions into their stories.

  3. All figures were based on website visits on April 19th, 2015.

  4. In a largely positive information environment, another mechanism that may decrease overall information search is suggested by Marcus and colleague’s “Affective Intelligence Theory” (Marcus et al. 2000). Affective Intelligence Theory proposes that a positive political context activates the brain’s “disposition” system, resulting in a decreased motivation to expand cognitive effort. In effect, positive signals tell the voter that the status quo is fine and there is no need to be on alert or vigilant. Thus, positive signals surrounding political information should decrease a person’s need to acquire information.

  5. We use a primary campaign to eliminate reliance on party identification when making a voting decision. In a primary, our subjects will presumably need more information to make their choices, giving us more leverage on our research question.

  6. The DPTE software is free to use and available to any researcher at www.processtracing.org.

  7. When we added a measure of political expertise to our models, subjects’ level of political sophistication had no substantive effects on the results reported here. Complete results are available from the authors.

  8. These and subsequent estimates from our statistical models are generated using the “sim” function for the Zelig statistical package in R (Imai et al. 2012).

  9. Recall that 22 of the 33 items pertaining to our manipulated candidate were marked with social cues. Of these, 11 items were initially presented with 20–30 likes or dislikes. The other 11 items contained 10–19 likes or dislikes and the final 11 items had no cues attached at all. The effects we report hold for both high and low–cued items, so we combine them here.

  10. While all subjects in our treatment groups were alerted to the presence of comments on a news item, in order to actually view the comments subjects had to choose to open a separate panel within the information box, much like visitors to Internet news sites must choose to read user comments.

  11. In the “negative + comments” treatment, the p value for the dummy variable is .051.

  12. A likely explanation for the increase in processing time is a “cognitive response” mechanism (Mutz 1998). Our subjects are probably taking the time to stop and think about the cues and what they imply. This interpretation is bolstered by the fact that subjects in the negative cues only group (i.e., no comments, only “dislikes”), also saw their processing time increase significantly.

  13. Although there were three candidates in the primary, our design focused the bulk of the cues on one politician. Our goal was to make one candidate appear to be either exceedingly popular or unpopular. The other two “non-manipulated” candidates received a smattering of cues simply to make the environment more realistic.

  14. To further investigate whether the distribution of cues mattered, we re-ran our analysis with dummy variables indicating whether the subject took the experiment early in the process (among the first third of subjects) or late (the last third). Since the later groups saw a different array of cues—because of the cues left by previous participants—we might see differences in information acquisition among these groups. However, the likelihood of opening an item declined for all subjects, regardless of when they took the study.

References

  • Banerjee, A. V. (1992). A simple model of herd behavior. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107, 797–817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (1996). Uninformed votes: Information effects in presidential elections. American Journal of Political Science, 40(1), 194–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2012). What makes online content viral? Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 192–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon. com’s mechanical turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpini, M. X. D., & Keeter, S. (1993). Measuring political knowledge: Putting first things first. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 1179–1206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. Journal of personality and social psychology, 85(5), 808–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dancey, L., & Sheagley, G. (2013). Heuristics behaving badly: Party cues and voter knowledge. American Journal of Political Science, 57(2), 312–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Peyton, B., & Verkuilen, J. (2007). Same facts, different interpretations: Partisan motivation and opinion on iraq. Journal of Politics, 69(4), 957–974.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, K., King, G., and Lau, O. (2012). Zelig: Everyone’s statistical software, R Package Version 3.5.5. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Zelig

  • Kuklinski, J. H., & Quirk, P. J. (2000). Reconsidering the rational public: Cognition, heuristics, and mass opinion. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of rationality (pp. 153–182). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Jerit, J., Schwieder, D., & Rich, R. F. (2000). Misinformation and the currency of democratic citizenship. Journal of Politics, 62(3), 790–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., Andersen, D. J., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2008). An exploration of correct voting in recent us presidential elections. American Journal of Political Science, 52(2), 395–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (1997). Voting correctly. American Political Science Review, 91, 585–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R. and Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and disadvantages of cognitive heuristics in political decision making. American Journal of Political Science, pages 951–971.

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). How voters decide: Information processing during election campaigns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lavine, H. G., Johnston, C. D., & Steenbergen, M. R. (2012). The ambivalent partisan: How critical loyalty promotes democracy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2000). Three steps toward a theory of motivated political reasoning. In A. Lupia, M. D. McCubbins, & S. L. Popkin (Eds.), Elements of Reason: Cognition, choice, and the bounds of rationality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2013). The rationalizing voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & MacKuen, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, M. L. (2005). Candidate occupations and voter information shortcuts. Journal of Politics, 67(1), 201–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muchnik, L., Aral, S., & Taylor, S. J. (2013). Social influence bias: A randomized experiment. Science, 341, 647–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mutz, D. C. (1998). Impersonal influence: How perceptions of mass collectives affect political attitudes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nyhan, B., & Reifler, J. (2010). When corrections fail: The persistence of political misperceptions. Political Behavior, 32(2), 303–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popkin, S. L. (1991). The reasoning voter: Communication and persuasion in presidential campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redlawsk, D. P. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. The Journal of Politics, 64(04), 1021–1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redlawsk, D. P., Civettini, A. J. W., & Emmerson, K. M. (2010). The affective tipping point: Do motivated reasoners ever âĂIJget itâĂİ? Political Psychology, 31(4), 563–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and choice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response: Answering questions versus revealing preferences. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Douglas R. Pierce.

Additional information

Financial support for this project comes from NSF Grant SoCS-IIS-096841 awarded to the second and third author. The development of the DPTE software system (www.processtracing.org) was supported in part by National Science Foundation grant SES-0647738 to the second author. Earlier versions of this paper were presented at a number of conferences, including the 2015 WPSA meeting and the 2015 MPSA conference. We thank participants at those seminars and a number of anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions and improvements. Replication data for this study is available at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataverse/polbehavior.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pierce, D.R., Redlawsk, D.P. & Cohen, W.W. Social Influences on Online Political Information Search and Evaluation. Polit Behav 39, 651–673 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9374-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9374-4

Keywords

Navigation