Skip to main content
Log in

Expressive Politics as (Costly) Norm Following

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When deciding whether to support a political candidate, policy or cause, individuals are observed to prioritize the expression of their political identities. They even knowingly incur personal costs (a lower wage, strained family relations) to do so. We argue that viewing political identities as social identities that impart norms on who or what one ought to support can help explain such costly political expression. Through population-based survey experiments, we show that individuals are aware of the norms attached to their political identities; will knowingly choose norm-compliance at personal cost; and that this costly political identity expression varies with norm salience and strength. Our results imply that as political identities strengthen, group norm compliance will increase, even at a cost, rendering compromise between political groups less likely.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Pickup, Mark, 2020, "Replication Data for: Expressive Politics as (Costly) Norm Following", https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/IJIBH3, Harvard Dataverse, V1.

  2. That said, the role of norms attached to other social identities, such as race (e.g., White et al. 2014) and non-political identities created in the lab (Bassi et al. 2011) has been tested. Further, the role of norms in voter turnout is well demonstrated (e.g., Bryan et al. 2011).

  3. We do not directly address competing identity-based norms with our experiments, but the implications can also shed light on how such trade-offs would be resolved.

  4. For many in the US, ideological and partisan identities have become so enmeshed (e.g., Mason 2018a)—with their liberal identity interchangeable with their Democrat identity or conservative identity interchangeable with Republican—that separating the norms of the two identities may not be practical.

  5. This is supported by evidence from Mason (2018b) that people readily recognize those on their own side of the spectrum as an in-group and those on the other side as an out-group. Our own data shows that approximately 78% in the US and 70% in Britain express an ideological identity and that it is stable over time (see Online Appendix 1).

  6. Kinder and Kalmoe (2017) also address this apparent inconsistency. Using observational data, they argue that although ideological identity is a cause of political behavior, it is mostly a consequence. We agree that it is both, but by using experimental data, we test how ideological identity can be an important cause of behavior.

  7. Information on the sampling procedures and field dates for this and the other studies are included in Online Appendix 3.

  8. The social issue items were adapted from the Wilson and Patterson (1968) conservatism measure.

  9. On the differences between measures of norms and personal preferences see: Groenendyk et al. (2020)

  10. Note: the differences are only statistically significant at the 0.05 level for about 1/3 of the issues for conservative, liberal and right-wing identifiers, and 2/3 of issues for left-wing identifiers.

  11. In Online Appendix 6, we explore the dimensions of the conservative/right-wing normative expectations and discover four, which we label fiscal, law and order, cultural, and religious. We also demonstrate that most ideologues have expectations in line with their group on most issues, and this is greatest for those with the strongest ideological identity.

  12. Bassi et al. (2011) also incentivize experimental subjects to vote against an identity group, but they used non-political identities created solely for the experiment. They therefore cannot manipulate pre-existing norms and test the norm mechanism.

  13. Study 2 has an oversample of left-wing identifiers. We estimate the oversampling to be 5 percentage points.

  14. Respondents had previously indicated whether they identified as ‘Very left-wing’ (6.8%), ‘Fairly left-wing’ (26.4%), ‘Slightly left-of-centre’ (28.5%), ‘Slightly right-of-centre’ (22.3%), ‘Fairly right-wing’ (13.2%), or ‘Very right-wing’ (2.8%). Those that answered ‘Centre’ were not included in the study.

  15. When right-wing Brits were asked about increased spending on social welfare in Study 1, the norm was unclear but here when it is paired with an increase in taxes, the norm becomes much clearer.

  16. We use a t-statistic. A reader may be concerned by the bimodal distribution of the data. However, given we have very large sample sizes, the sampling distribution for the (difference-of-means) test statistic should be normal and a t-statistic or z-score should be appropriate, regardless of the distribution of the data. This is borne out by the fact that the results are identical when using a nonparametric Fisher-Pitman permutation test. Tests of significance are one-tailed in accordance with our hypothesis.

  17. Tested using a difference-in-differences t-statistic.

  18. Online Appendix 9 provides additional details on how the treatments changed the distribution of responses.

  19. Online Appendix 9 provides additional details on how the treatments changed the distribution of responses.

References

  • Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (1990). Social identity theory: Constructive and critical advances. London: Harvester-Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achen, C. H., & Bartels, L. M. (2016). Democracy for realists: Why elections do not produce responsive government. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Akerlof, G. A., & Kranton, R. E. (2000). Economics and identity. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115, 715–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassi, A., Morton, R. B., & Williams, K. C. (2011). The effects of identities, incentives, and information on voting. Journal of Politics, 73(2), 558–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C. (2006). The grammar of society: The nature and dynamics of social norms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolsen, T., Druckman, J. N., & Cook, F. L. (2014). The influence of partisan motivated reasoning on public opinion. Political Behavior, 36, 235–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryan, C., Walton, G., Rogers, T., & Dweck, C. (2011). Motivating voter turnout by invoking the self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(31), 12653–12656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1981). The origins and meaning of the liberal/conservative self-identification. American Journal of Political Science, 25, 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and its discontents. New York: Glencoe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J., Kelly, K. A., & Weaver, K. (2001). Attitudes, norms, and social groups. In M. A. Hogg & S. Tyndale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Group processes. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, N. T. (2018). Identity sorting and political compromise. American Politics Research. https://doi.org/10.1177/1532673X18799273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., Reid, A., Mizrahi, K., & Ethier, K. A. (1995). Parameters of social identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(2), 280–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, C., & Stimson, J. A. (2012). Ideology in America. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. (1989). The Cement of Society: A study of social order. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Enelow, J. M., & Hinich, M. J. (1984). The spatial theory of voting. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S. (2003). Values, ideology, and the structure of political attitudes. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (p. 477–508). Oxford University Press.

  • Fiorina, M. P. (1976). The voting decision: Instrumental and expressive. Aspects Journal of Politics, 38(2), 390–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. P., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groenendyk, E. W. (2013). Competing motives in the partisan mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Groenendyk, E. W., Kimbrough, E. O., & Pickup, M. (2020). How norms shape the nature and origins of mass belief systems. SSRN. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3541289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin, A., & Jennings, C. (2019). Expressive voting. In R. D. Congleton, B. N. Grofman, & S. Voigt (Eds.), Oxford handbook of public choice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huddy, L. (2001). From social to political identity: A critical examination of social identity theory. Political Psychology, 22(1), 127–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huddy, L., Mason, L., & Aarøe, L. (2015). Expressive partisanship: Campaign involvement, political emotion, and partisan identity. American Political Science Review, 109(1), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, W. G. (2002). Liberal-conservative thinking in the American electorate. Micropolitics: Political Decision Making Participation, and Deliberation, 6, 97–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jessee, S. A. (2012). Ideology and spatial voting in American elections. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61(7), 651–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinder, D. R., & Kalmoe, N. P. (2017). Neither liberal nor conservative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klar, S. (2013). The influence of competing identity primes on political preferences. Journal of Politics, 75(4), 1108–1124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krupka, E., & Weber, R. (2013). Identifying social norms using coordination games: Why does dictator game sharing vary? Journal of the European Economic Association, 11(3), 495–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). How voters decide: Information processing in election campaigns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lenz, G. S. (2009). Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the priming hypothesis. American Journal of Political Science, 53, 821–837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levitan, L. C., & Verhulst, B. (2016). Conformity in groups: The effects of others’ views on expressed attitudes and attitude change. Political Behavior, 38(2), 277–315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-015-9312-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malka, A., & Lelkes, Y. (2010). More than ideology: Conservative-liberal identity and receptivity to political cues. Social Justice Research, 23, 156–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L. (2018a). Uncivil agreement: How politics became our identity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L. (2018b). Ideologues without issues: The polarizing consequences of ideological identities. Public Opinion Quarterly, 82(S1), 280–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, C., Margalit, Y., Malhotra, N., & Levendusky, M. (2018). The economic consequences of partisanship in a polarized era. American Journal of Political Science, 62, 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, P. J., Turner, J. C., & Haslam, S. A. (1991). Perceiving people as group members: The role of fit in the salience of social categorizations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 125–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, K. M., De Dreu, C. K. W., Schroth, H., Barry, B., Lituchy, T. R., & Bazerman, H. (2002). What we want to do versus what we think we should do: An empirical investigation of intrapersonal conflict. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(5), 403–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popkin, S. L. (1991). The reasoning voter: Communication and persuasion in presidential campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Redlawsk, D. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration. The Journal of Politics, 64, 1021–1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheff, T. J. (1988). Shame and conformity: The difference-emotion system. American Sociological Review, 53(3), 395–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (1953). Groups in harmony and tension. New York: Octagon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, B. (2012). The social citizen. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., & Stigliltz, E. H. (2012). Reputational premium. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Suhay, E. (2015). Explaining group influence: The role of identity and emotion in political conformity and polarization. Political Behavior, 37, 221–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 223(5), 96–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H. (1978). Social categorization, social identity, and social comparisons. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (2nd ed.). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toff, B., & Suhay, E. (2018). Partisan conformity, social identity, and the formation of policy preferences. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 31(2), 349–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior. In E. J. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group processes. Greenwich: JAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Cambridge: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, E. A., & Iyengar, S. (2020). Partisanship as a social identity: Implications for polarization. Political Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09637-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, I., Laird, C., & Allen, T. (2014). Selling out? The politics of navigating conflicts between racial group interest and self-interest. American Political Science Review, 108(4), 783–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 264–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ybarra, O., & Trafimow, D. (1998). How priming the collective self affects the relative weights of attitudes and subjective norms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 362–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Grant 435-2016-1173. We thank seminar audiences at the Canadian Political Science Association, the American Political Science Association, Toronto Political Behaviour Workshop, Stony Brook University, the 2018 IFREE Graduate Student Workshop, the University of Michigan, the University of California-Irvine, and the University of Dayton for helpful comments. We also benefitted from discussions with Eric Groenendyk, J. Scott Matthews, Eric Guntermann, Stanley Feldman, Leonie Huddy, and Michelangelo Landgrave. All remaining errors are our own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark Pickup.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 14524 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pickup, M., Kimbrough, E.O. & de Rooij, E.A. Expressive Politics as (Costly) Norm Following. Polit Behav 44, 1611–1631 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09667-6

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09667-6

Keywords

Navigation