Skip to main content
Log in

A review of reading prosody acquisition and development

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present work reviews the current knowledge of the development of reading prosody, or reading aloud with expression, in young children. Prosody comprises the variables of timing, phrasing, emphasis and intonation that speakers use to convey meaning. We detail the subjective rating scales proposed as a means of assessing performance in young readers and the objective features of each as markers of progress. Finally, we review studies that have explored the intricate relations between automaticity, prosody and comprehension.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Álvarez-Cañizo, M., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2015). The role of reading fluency in children’s text comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology,6, 1810.

    Google Scholar 

  • Álvarez-Cañizo, M., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2017). Reading prosody development in Spanish children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,31, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arcand, M.-S., Dion, E., Lemire-Théberge, L., Guay, M.-H., Barrette, A., Gagnon, V., et al. (2014). Segmenting texts into meaningful word groups: Beginning readers’ prosody and comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading,18, 208–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailly, G., & Gouvernayre, C. (2012). Pauses and respiratory markers of the structure of book reading. Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association,13, 2218–2221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailly, G., Rochet-Capellan, A., & Vilain, C. (2013). Adaptation of respiratory patterns in collaborative reading. Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association,14, 1653–1657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beinum, F. J. K. (1991). Spectro-temporal reduction and expansion in spontaneous speech and read text: Focus words versus non-focus words. Phonetics and Phonology of Speaking Styles,1, 361–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, R. G., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2010). Text complexity and oral reading prosody in young readers. Reading Research Quarterly,45, 388–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, R. G., Schwanenflugel, P. J., Meisinger, E. B., Groff, C., Kuhn, M. R., & Steiner, L. (2013). A spectrographically grounded scale for evaluating reading expressiveness. Reading Research Quarterly,48, 105–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, K. S., Tighe, E., Jiang, Y., Kaftanski, K., Qi, C., & Ardoin, S. P. (2013). Reading expressively and understanding thoroughly: An examination of prosody in adults with low literacy skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,26, 665–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D., & Bolinger, D. L. M. (1989). Intonation and its uses: Melody in grammar and discourse. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calet, N., Gutiérrez-Palma, N., & Defior, S. (2015a). A cross-sectional study of fluency and reading comprehension in Spanish primary school children. Journal of Research in Reading,38, 272–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calet, N., Gutiérrez-Palma, N., Simpson, I. C., González-Trujillo, M. C., & Defior, S. (2015b). Suprasegmental phonology development and reading acquisition: A longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading,19, 51–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. (1988). Punctuation and the prosody of written language. Written Communication,5, 395–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowie, R., Douglas-Cowie, E., & Wichmann, A. (2002). Prosodic characteristics of skilled reading: Fluency and expressiveness in 8 to 10-year-old readers. Language and Speech,45, 47–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., Martínez-García, C., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (2018). Prosodic perception problems in spanish dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading,22, 41–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1996). Comment la parole vient aux enfants (How infants learn to speak). Paris: Odile Jacob.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Boysson-Bardies, B., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1984). Discernible differences in the babbling of infants according to target language. Journal of Child Language,11, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dellwo, V., Leemann, A., & Kolly, M.-J. (2015). The recognition of read and spontaneous speech in local vernacular: The case of Zurich German. Journal of Phonetics,48, 13–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickens, R. H., & Meisinger, E. B. (2016). Examining the effects of skill level and reading modality on reading comprehension. Reading Psychology,37(2), 318–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodane, C., & Brunellière, A. (2006). Lecture silencieuse et oralisée des phrases relatives: le rôle de la prosodie (Silent and oral reading of relational sentences: The fonction of prosody). Actes de conférence Journées d’études sur la parole,26, 117–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dowhower, S. L. (1991). Speaking of prosody: Fluency’s unattended bedfellow. Theory into Practice,30, 165–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erekson, J. A. (2010). Prosody and interpretation. Reading Horizons, 50, 3. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/reading_horizons/vol50/iss2/3. Accessed May 2018.

  • Fernandes, S., Querido, L., Verhaeghe, A., & Araújo, L. (2018). What is the relationship between reading prosody and reading comprehension in european portuguese? Evidence from grades 2 to 5. Journal of Research in Reading,41, S102–S129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filipe, M. G., Peppé, S., Frota, S., & Vicente, S. G. (2017). Prosodic development in European Portuguese from childhood to adulthood. Applied Psycholinguistics,38, 1045–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., Carlson, K., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2006). Prosodic phrasing is central to language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,10, 244–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Godde, E., Bailly, G., Escudero, D., Bosse, M.-L., & Gillet-Perret, E. (2017). Evaluation of reading performance of primary school children: Objective measurements vs. subjective ratings. In International workshop on child computer interaction (WOCCI) (Vol. 1, pp. 23–27).

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2006). The Cambridge mindreading (cam) face-voice battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,36, 169–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, J.-P., Auchlin, A., & Simon, A. C. (2009). Discrimination de styles de parole par analyse prosodique semi-automatique (Speech style jugement using semi-automatic prosodic analysis). Actes d’IDP,1, 207–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Fosker, T., Huss, M., Mead, N., & Szűcs, D. (2011). Rise time and formant transition duration in the discrimination of speech sounds: The ba–wa distinction in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Science,14, 34–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,99, 10911–10916.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F., & Collins, M. (1979). Breathing, pausing and reading. Phonetica,36, 98–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guaïtella, I. (1999). Rhythm in speech: What rhythmic organizations reveal about cognitive processes in spontaneous speech production versus reading aloud. Journal of Pragmatics,31, 509–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (2013). Language experience and early language development: From input to uptake. Erlbaum: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haskins, T., & Aleccia, V. (2014). Toward a reliable measure of prosody: An investigation of rater consistency. International Journal of Education and Social Science,1, 102–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschberg, J. (2000). A corpus-based approach to the study of speaking style. In M. Horne (Ed.), Prosody: Theory and experiment (pp. 335–350). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, D., & Di Cristo, A. (1998). Intonation systems: A survey of twenty languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holliman, A. J. (2014). Speech rhythm sensitivity in pre-readers: What role does it have in reading acquisition? London: Nuffield Foundation. Retrieved from www.nuffieldfoundation.org. Accessed July 2018.

  • Holliman, A. J., Palma, N. G., Critten, S., Wood, C., Cunnane, H., & Pillinger, C. (2017). Examining the independent contribution of prosodic sensitivity to word reading and spelling in early readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,30, 509–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, P., & Kadi-Hanifi, K. (1991). Comparison of prosodic properties between read and spontaneous speech material. Speech Communication,10, 163–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N., Cuetos, F., & Suarez-Coalla, P. (2019). Prosody in the reading of children with specific language impairment. Infancia y Aprendizaje,42, 87–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kentner, G., & Vasishth, S. (2016). Prosodic focus marking in silent reading: Effects of discourse context and rhythm. Frontiers in Psychology,7, 319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klauda, S. L., & Guthrie, J. T. (2008). Relationships of three components of reading fluency to reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology,100, 310–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Kreiner, H., & Greenberg, S. N. (2002). The extraction of structure during reading: Evidence from reading prosody. Memory & Cognition,30, 270–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Reading Research Quarterly,45, 230–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lai, S. A., George Benjamin, R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Kuhn, M. R. (2014). The longitudinal relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension skills in second-grade children. Reading & Writing Quarterly,30, 116–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalain, M., Espesser, R., Ghio, A., De Looze, C., Reis, C., & Mendonca-Alves, L. (2014). Prosodie et lecture: Particularités temporelles et mélodiques de l’enfant dyslexique en lecture et en narration (Reading and prosody: Temporal and melodic characteristics of dyslexic children in reading and speech). Revue de Laryngologie Otologie Rhinologie,135, 71–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalain, M., Legou, T., Fauth, C., Hirsch, F., & Didirkova, I. (2016). Que disent nos silences? Apport des données acoustiques, articulatoires et physiologiques pour l’étude des pauses silencieuses (What do our silences tell? Contribution of accoustic, articulatory and physiological data to study silent pauses). Actes de la conférence conjointe JEP-TALN-RECITAL,2016(1), 563–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalain, M., Mendonça-Alves, L., Espesser, R., Ghio, A., De Looze, C., & Reis, C. (2012). Lecture et prosodie chez l’enfant dyslexique, le cas des pauses (Reading and Prosody in dyslexic children, pauses issues). Actes de conférence Journées d’Etudes sur la Parole,29, 41–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopes, J. A., Silva, M. M., Moniz, A. V., Spear-Swerling, L., & Zibulsky, J. (2015). Prosody growth and reading comprehension: A longitudinal study from 2nd through the end of 3rd grade. Revista de Psicodidáctica/Journal of Psychodidactics,20, 5–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, P. (2011). Ponctuation et structure prosodique (Punctuation and prosodic structure). Langue Française,4(172), 99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meisinger, E. B., Bradley, B. A., Schwanenflugel, P. J., Kuhn, M. R., & Morris, R. D. (2009). Myth and reality of the word caller: The relation between teacher nominations and prevalence among elementary school children. School Psychology Quarterly,24, 147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2006). prosody of syntactically complex sentences in the oral reading of young children. Journal of Educational Psychology,98, 839–853.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2008). A longitudinal study of the development of reading prosody as a dimension of oral reading fluency in early elementary school children. Reading Research Quarterly,43, 336–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, J. L., & Demuth, K. (2014). Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. New-York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moser, G. P., Sudweeks, R. R., Morrison, T. G., & Wilcox, B. (2014). Reliability of ratings of children’s expressive reading. Reading Psychology,35, 58–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, M., Iwano, K., & Furui, S. (2008). Differences between acoustic characteristics of spontaneous and read speech and their effects on speech recognition performance. Computer Speech & Language,22, 171–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence based assessment of the scientific literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. G. K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Cassidy, K. W. (1989). How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. Journal of child Language,16, 55–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paige, D. D., Rasinski, T. V., & Magpuri-Lavell, T. (2012). Is fluent, expressive reading important for high school readers? Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy,56, 67–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paige, D. D., Rasinski, T., Magpuri-Lavell, T., & Smith, G. S. (2014). Interpreting the relationships among prosody, automaticity, accuracy, and silent reading comprehension in secondary students. Journal of Literacy Research,46, 123–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paige, D. D., Rupley, W. H., Smith, G. S., Rasinski, T. V., Nichols, W., & Magpuri-Lavell, T. (2017). Is prosodic reading a strategy for comprehension? Journal for Educational Research Online/Journal für Bildungsforschung Online,9, 245–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinnell, G. S., Pikulski, J. J., Wixson, K. K., Campbell, J. R., Gough, P. B., & Beatty, A. S. (1995). Listening to children read aloud: Data from NAEP’s integrated reading performance record (IRPR) at grade 4. The Nation’s Report Card. Report No 23-FR-04. Washington, DC: Office of Educational Research and Improvement, U.S. Department of Education.

  • Price, K. W., Meisinger, E. B., Louwerse, M. M., & D’Mello, S. (2016). The contributions of oral and silent reading fluency to reading comprehension. Reading Psychology,37, 167–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prieto, P., & Esteve-Gibert, N. (2018). The development of prosody in first language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prior, S. M., Fenwick, K. D., Saunders, K. S., Ouellette, R., O’Quinn, C., & Harvey, S. (2011). Comprehension after oral and silent reading: Does grade level matter? Literacy Research and Instruction,50, 183–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. V. (2004). Assessing reading fluency. Honolulu: Pacific Resources for Education and Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. V. (2006). Reading fluency instruction: Moving beyond accuracy, automaticity, and prosody. The Reading Teacher,59, 704–706.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. V. (2010). The fluent reader: Oral & silent reading strategies for building fluency, word recognition & comprehension. New York, NY: Scholastic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. V., & Cheesman Smith, M. (2018). The megabook of fluency. New York, NY: Scholastic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. V., Padak, N. D., McKeon, C. A., Wilfong, L. G., Friedauer, J. A., & Heim, P. (2005). Is reading fluency a key for successful high school reading? Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy,49, 22–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T., Rikli, A., & Johnston, S. (2009). Reading fluency: More than automaticity? more than a concern for the primary grades? Literacy Research and Instruction,48, 350–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Mashraki, Y. E. (2007). Prosodic reading, reading comprehension and morphological skills in hebrew-speaking fourth graders. Journal of Research in Reading,30, 140–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, M. (1980). Le français, langue sans accent? (Is French a language without accentuation?). Studia Phonetica Montréal,15, 13–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmel, N., & Ness, M. (2017). The effects of oral and silent reading on reading comprehension. Reading Psychology,38, 390–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, P. A. (1980). On the acquisition of reading fluency. Journal of Reading Behavior,12, 177–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, P. A. (1987). Prosody and structure in children’s syntactic processing. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 243–270). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, P. A. (1991). Understanding prosody’s role in reading acquisition. Theory into Practice,30, 158–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Benjamin, R. G. (2012). Reading experssiveness: The neglected aspect of reading fluency. In T. Rasinski, C. Blachowicz, & K. Lems (Eds.), Fluency instruction: Research based best practice (pp. 35–54). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Benjamin, R. G. (2016). The development of reading prosody and its assessment. In J. Thomson & L. Jarmulowicz (Eds.), Linguistic rhythm and literacy (pp. 187–213). Amsterdam: John Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Benjamin, R. G. (2017). Lexical prosody as an aspect of oral reading fluency. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,30, 143–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Hamilton, A. M., Kuhn, M. R., Wisenbaker, J. M., & Stahl, S. A. (2004). Becoming a fluent reader: Reading skill and prosodic features in the oral reading of young readers. Journal of Educational Psychology,96, 119–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Westmoreland, M. R., & Benjamin, R. G. (2015). Reading fluency skill and the prosodic marking of linguistic focus. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,28, 9–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in european orthographies. British Journal of Psychology,94, 143–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suàrez-Coalla, P., Álvarez-Cañizo, M., Martinez, C., Garcia, N., & Cuetos, F. (2016). Reading prosody in Spanish dyslexics. Annals of dyslexia,66, 275–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valencia, S. W., Smith, A. T., Reece, A. M., Li, M., Wixson, K. K., & Newman, H. (2010). Oral Reading Fluency Assessment: Issues of construct, criterion, and consequential validity. Reading Research Quarterly,45, 270–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenendaal, N. J., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2014). The role of speech prosody and text reading prosody in children’s reading comprehension. British Journal of Educational Psychology,84, 521–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenendaal, N. J., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2016a). Bidirectional relations between text reading prosody and reading comprehension in the upper primary school grades: A longitudinal perspective. Scientific Studies of Reading,20, 189–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenendaal, N. J., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2016b). The contribution of segmental and suprasegmental phonology to reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly,51, 55–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, B., Peppé, S., & Goulandris, N. (2004). Intonation development from five to thirteen. Journal of Child Language,31, 749–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wennerstrom, A. (2001). The music of everyday speech: Prosody and discourse analysis. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C., Wade-Woolley, L., & Holliman, A. J. (2009). Phonological awareness. In C. Wood & V. Connelly (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on reading and spelling (pp. 7–23). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yildirim, K., Rasinski, T., & Kaya, D. (2018). Fluency and comprehension of narrative texts in Turkish students in grades 4 through 8. Education,3–13(47), 348–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yildiz, M., Yildirim, K., Ates, S., Rasinski, T., Fitzgerald, S., & Zimmerman, B. (2014). The relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension in fifth-grade Turkish students. International Journal of School & Educational Psychology,2, 35–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. R., & Bowers, P. G. (1995). Individual difference and text difficulty determinant of reading fluency and expressiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,60, 428–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, C., & Rasinski, T. (2018). Readers Theatre: Effects on word recognition automaticity and reading prosody. Journal of Research in Reading,41, 475–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zutell, J., & Rasinski, T. V. (1991). Training teachers to attend to their students’ oral reading fluency. Theory into Practice,30, 211–217.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Funding was provided by Caisse des dépots et consignation (E-FRAN PIA program).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erika Godde.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

This research has been conducted as part of “Fluence”, an e-FRAN project founded by the “Investissement d’Avenir” program handled by the French “Caisse des dépôts”.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Godde, E., Bosse, ML. & Bailly, G. A review of reading prosody acquisition and development. Read Writ 33, 399–426 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09968-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09968-1

Keywords

Navigation