Skip to main content
Log in

Language switching costs in bilingual mathematics learning: Transfer effects and individual differences

Sprachwechselkosten beim zweisprachigen Mathematiklernen: Transfereffekte und individuelle Unterschiede

  • Schwerpunkt
  • Published:
Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is an implicit assumption in Content- and Language Integrated Learning that the acquired knowledge is represented independently of the language of instruction. However, it could be shown in several experimental intervention studies that cognitive costs (i. e. longer reaction times and lower accuracy) arise when the languages of study and retrieval do not match. In the present study, we focused on arithmetic fact learning and investigated whether these cognitive costs generalize to more complex contexts. In addition, we explored the relationship between the cognitive costs and individual differences in executive functions, intelligence, mathematical competence and second language (L2) proficiency. Participants were 58 German-French bilingual university students (L2 proficiency B2 or above). They studied multiplication facts for 3 consecutive days in either their L1 or L2, followed by a test in both languages on the 4th day. Cognitive costs caused by language switching between training and test were found for both problems requiring simple fact retrieval and problems requiring knowledge application in novel, more complex text problems. The costs were negatively related with L2 proficiency and positively with inhibition. This study shows for the first time that language switching costs can be found in situations when knowledge needs to be applied in a new context, as it is often necessary in classroom learning. Implications of this study will be discussed with regards to bilingual arithmetic learning.

Zusammenfassung

Bei fremdsprachlichem Fachunterricht wird implizit davon ausgegangen, dass das erworbene Wissen unabhängig von der Instruktionssprache repräsentiert ist. In mehreren experimentellen Interventionsstudien konnte allerdings gezeigt werden, dass kognitive Kosten in Form von längeren Reaktionszeiten und höheren Fehlerzahlen entstehen, wenn die Sprache beim Wissensabruf nicht mit der Instruktionssprache übereinstimmt. In der vorliegenden Studie wurde für arithmetisches Faktenlernen untersucht, ob sich diese Kosten auf komplexere Aufgaben übertragen. Weiterhin wurde deren Zusammenhang mit exekutiven Funktionen, Intelligenz, Rechen- und Sprachkompetenzen in der Zweitsprache (L2) geprüft. Die Studienteilnehmer waren 58 zweisprachige (deutsch-französisch) Studierende (L2-Sprachfähigkeiten B2 oder höher). Sie lernten die Ergebnisse von Multiplikationsaufgaben über 3 aufeinanderfolgende Tage in der L1 oder L2, mit einem anschließenden Test auf beiden Sprachen am 4. Tag. Kognitive Kosten durch Sprachwechsel zwischen Training und Testung wurden für die Reaktionszeiten bei Aufgaben, die einen einfachen Faktenabruf erforderten, sowie bei Transferaufgaben, die Anwendung des Wissens in einem neuen, komplexeren Kontext erfordern, gefunden. Das Ausmaß der Kosten zeigte negative Zusammenhänge mit den Sprachfähigkeiten in L2 und positive Zusammenhänge mit Inhibition. Mit dieser Studie konnte erstmalig gezeigt werden, dass kognitive Kosten auch in Situationen entstehen, in denen Gelerntes in einen neuen Kontext gestellt wird, wie es im Rahmen schulischen Lernens erforderlich ist. Implikationen der Studie werden im Hinblick auf zweisprachiges Mathematiklernen diskutiert.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abedi, J., & Lord, C. (2001). The language factor in mathematics tests. Applied Measurement in Education, 14(3), 219–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Abreu, E. P. M. J., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1364–1371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergström, K., Klatte, M., Steinbrink, C., & Lachmann, T. (2016). First and second language acquisition in German children attending a kindergarten immersion program: a combined longitudinal and cross-sectional study. Language Learning, 66(2), 386–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(1), 68–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: the good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(01), 3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: the role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 117–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7(3), 325–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24(1), 40–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blum, W., & Niss, M. (1991). Applied mathematical problem solving, modelling, applications, and links to other subjects ?: State, trends and issues in mathematics instruction. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22(1), 37–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M. (2013). LexTALE_FR: A fast, free, and efficient test to measure language proficiency in French. Psychologica Belgica, 53(1), 23–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, L., Li, M., Kirby, J. R., Qiang, H., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2010). English language immersion and students’ academic achievement in English, Chinese and mathematics. Evaluation & Research in Education, 23(3), 151–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Council for Cultural Co-operation (2009). Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment (10th edn.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Council of Europe (2002). Common European framework of reference for languages: learning, teaching, assessment; case studies. Language policies. Strasbourg: Council of Europe Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crystal, D. (2008). Two thousand million? English Today, 24(01), 3–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dallinger, S., Jonkmann, K., Hollm, J., & Fiege, C. (2016). The effect of content and language integrated learning on students’ English and history competences—Killing two birds with one stone? Learning and Instruction, 41, 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dowker, A. (2005). Individual differences in arithmetic: Implications for psychology, neuroscience and education. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • EACEA, Eurydice, & Eurostat (2012). Key data on teaching languages at school in Europe. Brussels: Eurydice.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests

    Google Scholar 

  • Eurydice (2006). Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL) at School in Europe. Brussels: Eurydice.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2013). Early foundations for mathematics learning and their relations to disabilities. Current directions in psychological science, 22(1), 23–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Language in mind: advances in the study of language and thought. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabner, R. H., & De Smedt, B. (2012). Oscillatory EEG correlates of arithmetic strategies: a training study. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(428), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabner, R. H., Ischebeck, A., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., Delazer, M., Ebner, F., & Neuper, C. (2009). Fact learning in complex arithmetic and figural-spatial tasks: the role of the angular gyrus and its relation to mathematical competence. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2936–2952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grabner, R. H., Saalbach, H., & Eckstein, D. (2012). Language-switching costs in bilingual mathematics learning. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6(3), 147–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(02), 67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gumperz, J. J., & Levinson, S. C. (1996). Rethinking linguistic relativity

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 18(4), 625–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iluz-Cohen, P., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2013). Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(04), 884–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., Schalk, L., Saalbach, H., & Okada, H. (2014). All giraffes have female-specific properties: influence of grammatical gender on deductive reasoning about sex-specific properties in German speakers. Cognitive science, 38(3), 514–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jäger, A. O., Süß, H.-M., & Beauducel, A. (1997). Berliner Intelligenzstruktur-Test: BIS-Test Form 4. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jäppinen, A. K. (2005). Thinking and content learning of mathematics and science as cognitional development in content and language integrated learning (CLIL): teaching through a foreign language in Finland. Language and Education, 19(2), 147–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Developmental Science, 12(6), 978–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempert, S., Saalbach, H., & Hardy, I. (2011). Cognitive benefits and costs of bilingualism in elementary school students: the case of mathematical word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 547–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lo, Y. Y., & Lo, E. S. C. (2014). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of English-medium education in Hong Kong. Review of Educational Research, 84(1), 47–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, J. F. (1959). Paced memorizing in a continuous task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(3), 206–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malt, B. C., & Wolff, P. (2010). Words and the mind: how words capture human experience.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marian, V., & Fausey, C. M. (2006). Language-dependent memory in bilingual learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(8), 1025–1047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(01), 81–93. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. F., & Stigler, J. W. (1987). Counting in Chinese: cultural variation in a basic cognitive skill. Cognitive Development, 2(3), 279–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J., & Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: the role of number-naming systems. Psychological Science, 6(1), 56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K., Hilchey, M. D., Singh, N., & Klein, R. M. (2012). On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement. The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 65(8), 1502–1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Möller, J., Hohenstein, F., Fleckenstein, J., & Baumert, J. (2017). Formen und Effekte des Fremdsprachenerwerbs und der bilingualen Beschulung. In J. Möller, F. Hohenstein, J. Fleckenstein, O. Köller, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Erfolgreich integrieren – die Staatliche Europa-Schule Berlin. Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paetsch, J., Felbrich, A., & Stanat, P. (2015). Der Zusammenhang von sprachlichen und mathematischen Kompetenzen bei Kindern mit Deutsch als Zweitsprache. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 29(1), 19–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piesche, N., Jonkmann, K., Fiege, C., & Keßler, J. U. (2016). CLIL for all? A randomised controlled field experiment with sixth-grade students on the effects of content and language integrated science learning. Learning and Instruction, 44, 108–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saalbach, H., Imai, M., & Schalk, L. (2012). Grammatical gender and inferences about biological properties in german-speaking children. Cognitive science, 36(7), 1251–1267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saalbach, H., Eckstein, D., Andri, N., Hobi, R., & Grabner, R. H. (2013). When language of instruction and language of application differ: cognitive costs of bilingual mathematics learning (PSYNDEXshort). Learning and Instruction, 26, 36–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saalbach, H., Gunzenhauser, C., Kempert, S., & Karbach, J. (2016). Der Einfluss von Mehrsprachigkeit auf mathematische Fähigkeiten bei Grundschulkindern mit niedrigem sozioökonomischen Status. Frühe Bildung, 5(2), 73–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of applied psychology, 51(3), 300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., & Tsivkin, S. (2001). Language and number: a bilingual training study. Cognition, 78(1), 45–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turnbull, M., Hart, D., & Lapkin, S. (2003). Grade 6 French immersion students’ performance on large-scale reading, writing, and mathematics tests: building explanations. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 49(1), 6–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., & de Corte, E. (2002). Everyday knowledge and mathematical modeling of school word problems. In K. Gravemeijer, R. Lehrer, B. van Oers, & L. Verschaffel (Eds.), Symbolizing, modeling and tool use in mathematics education (pp. 257–276). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Yan, S., & Nicoladis, E. (2009). Finding le mot juste: differences between bilingual and monolingual children’s lexical access in comprehension and production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(03), 323–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaunbauer, A. C. M., & Möller, J. (2010). Schulleistungsentwicklung immersiv unterrichteter Grundschüler in den ersten zwei Schuljahren. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 57(1), 30–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zied, K. M., Phillipe, A., Karine, P., Valerie, H. T., Ghislaine, A., & Arnaud, R. (2004). Bilingualism and adult differences in inhibitory mechanisms: evidence from a bilingual Stroop task. Brain and cognition, 54(3), 254–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Henrik Saalbach.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 6 Intercorrelations for all variables, German and French Training, n = 58

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Volmer, E., Grabner, R.H. & Saalbach, H. Language switching costs in bilingual mathematics learning: Transfer effects and individual differences. Z Erziehungswiss 21, 71–96 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-017-0795-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-017-0795-6

Keywords

Schlüsselwörter

Navigation