Skip to main content
Log in

Neuroanatomical substrates accounting for the effect of present hedonistic time perspective on risk preference: the mediating role of right posterior parietal cortex

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Brain Imaging and Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The preference for taking risk troubles people across multiple domains including health, economics, and social well-being. Prior research has demonstrated that risk preference can be influenced by time perspective (TP). However, little is known about the neural substrates underlying the effect of TP on risk preference. Here, we used a voxel-based morphometry (VBM) method across two samples to address this question. In Sample 1, the behavioral results showed a positive correlation between present hedonistic TP (PHTP) and gambling rate (the index of risk preference), indicating the higher PHTP, the greater the preference for risk. Subsequently, the whole-brain VBM results found that gambling rate was negatively correlated with the gray matter (GM) volume of a cluster in the right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC). The PHTP score was also negatively related to the GM volume of another cluster in the rPPC. We then examined an overlapping region in the rPPC using a conjunction analysis method. The GM volume of this overlapping brain region was related to both PHTP score and gambling rate. Finally, the mediation analysis found that the GM volume of overlapping region in rPPC played a role in explaining the effect of PHTP on risk preference. This result was also reproduced and validated in another independent sample. Taken together, our findings manifest that the structural variation of rPPC can account for the influence that PHTP has upon the risk preference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abdel-khalik, & Rashad, A. (2014). Prospect theory predictions in the field: Risk seekers in settings of weak accounting controls. Journal of Accounting Literature, 33(1–2), 58–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, J. (2009). The role of time perspective in smoking cessation amongst older English adults. Health Psychology, 28(5), 529.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, J., & Nettle, D. (2009). Time perspective, personality and smoking, body mass, and physical activity: An empirical study. British Journal of Health Psychology, 14(1), 83–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alvos, L., Gregson, R., & Ross, M. W. (1993). Future time perspective in current and previous injecting drug users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 31(2), 193–197.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, R. A., & Buneo, C. A. (2002). Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25(1), 189–220.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apostolidis, T., Fieulaine, N., Simonin, L., & Rolland, G. (2006). Cannabis use, time perspective and risk perception: Evidence of a moderating effect. Psychology and Health, 21(5), 571–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, K. M., McDermott, K. B., & Szpunar, K. K. (2011). Individual differences in time perspective predict autonoetic experience. Consciousness & Cognition, 20(3), 712–719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 115–116.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage, 38(1), 95–113.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry—The methods. Neuroimage, 11(6), 805–821.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ballard, K., & Knutson, B. (2009). Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage, 45(1), 143–150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barraclough, D. J., Conroy, M. L., & Lee, D. (2004). Prefrontal cortex and decision making in a mixed-strategy game. Nature Neuroscience, 7(4), 404–410.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beenstock, J., Adams, J., & White, M. (2010). The association between time perspective and alcohol consumption in university students: Cross-sectional study. The European journal of public health, 21(4), 438–443.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bellgrove, M. A., Hester, R., & Garavan, H. (2004). The functional neuroanatomical correlates of response variability: Evidence from a response inhibition task. Neuropsychologia, 42(14), 1910–1916.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boniwell, I., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Balancing time perspective in pursuit of optimal functioning. Positive psychology in practice, 12, 141–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, J. N., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2006). Time perspective, health, and risk taking Understanding behavior in the context of time (pp. 97-119): Psychology Press.

  • Chen, T., Liu, L.-l., Cui, J.-f., Chen, X.-j., Wang, J., Zhang, Y.-b., . . . Shum, D. H. (2016). Present-fatalistic time perspective and life satisfaction: The moderating role of age. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 161–165.

  • Chen, Z., Guo, Y., & Feng, T. (2018). Neuroanatomical correlates of time perspective: A voxel-based morphometry study. Behavioural Brain Research, 339.

  • Christopoulos, G. I., Tobler, P. N., Bossaerts, P., Dolan, R. J., & Schultz, W. (2009). Neural correlates of value, risk, and risk aversion contributing to decision making under risk. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(40), 12574–12583.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Studer, B., Bruss, J., Tranel, D., & Bechara, A. (2014). Damage to insula abolishes cognitive distortions during simulated gambling. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(16), 6098–6103.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cosenza, M., & Nigro, G. (2015). Wagering the future: Cognitive distortions, impulsivity, delay discounting, and time perspective in adolescent gambling. Journal of Adolescence, 45, 56–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cosenza, M., Griffiths, M. D., Nigro, G., & Ciccarelli, M. (2017). Risk-taking, delay discounting, and time perspective in adolescent gamblers: An experimental study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(2), 383–395.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daugherty, J. R., & Brase, G. L. (2010). Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality & Individual Differences, 48(2), 202–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2005). Neurobiology of decision making: A selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 58(8), 597–604.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Munson, S., Eshel, N., et al. (2004). Choice selection and reward anticipation: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 42(12), 1585–1597.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fieulaine, N., & Martinez, F. (2010). Time under control: Time perspective and desire for control in substance use. Addictive Behaviors, 35(8), 799–802.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fromme, K., Katz, E., & D'Amico, E. (1997). Effects of alcohol intoxication on the perceived consequences of risk taking. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5(1), 14–23.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fu, L. P., Bi, G. H., Zou, Z. T., Wang, Y., Ye, E. M., Ma, L., Ming-Fan, & Yang, Z. (2008). Impaired response inhibition function in abstinent heroin dependents: An fMRI study. Neuroscience Letters, 438(3), 322–326.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilaie-Dotan, S., Tymula, A., Cooper, N., Kable, J. W., Glimcher, P. W., & Levy, I. (2014). Neuroanatomy predicts individual risk attitudes. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(37), 12394–12401.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grable, J. E. (2000). Financial risk tolerance and additional factors that affect risk taking in everyday money matters. Journal of Business and Psychology, 14(4), 625–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grubb, M. A., Tymula, A., Gilaie-Dotan, S., Glimcher, P. W., & Levy, I. (2016). Neuroanatomy accounts for age-related changes in risk preferences. Nature Communications, 7, 13822.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J., Cunningham, W. A., Kirkland, T., & Hay, A. C. (2012). Feeling stuck in the present? Mania proneness and history associated with present-oriented time perspective. Emotion, 12(1), 13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayasaka, S., Phan, K. L., Liberzon, I., Worsley, K. J., & Nichols, T. E. (2004). Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage, 22(2), 676–687.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Scharkow, M. (2013). The relative trustworthiness of inferential tests of the indirect effect in statistical mediation analysis: Does method really matter? Psychological science, 24(10), 1918-1927. 

  • Hodgins, D. C., & Engel, A. (2002). Future time perspective in pathological gamblers. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190(11), 775–780.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huettel, & A, S. (2005). Decisions under uncertainty: Probabilistic context influences activation of prefrontal and parietal cortices. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(13), 3304–3311.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huettel, S. A., Gordon, A. E., Warner, B. B., Platt, H. L., & Stowe, J. C. (2006). Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and ambiguity. Neuron, 49(5), 765–775.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huk, A. C., & Meister, M. L. R. (2012). Neural correlates and neural computations in posterior parietal cortex during perceptual decision-making. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 86.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jochemczyk, Ł., Pietrzak, J., Buczkowski, R., Stolarski, M., & Markiewicz, Ł. (2017). You only live once: Present-hedonistic time perspective predicts risk propensity. Personality and Individual Differences, 115, 148–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 10(12), 1625–1633.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2000). Choices, values and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keough, K. A., Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Who's smoking, drinking, and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(2), 149–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koehler, S., Hasselmann, E., Wüstenberg, T., Heinz, A., & Romanczuk-Seiferth, N. (2015). Higher volume of ventral striatum and right prefrontal cortex in pathological gambling. Brain Structure and Function, 220(1), 469–477.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (2009). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology: Macmillan.

  • Leon, M. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2003). Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron, 38(2), 317–327.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, I. (2017). Neuroanatomical substrates for risk behavior. The Neuroscientist, 23(3), 275–286.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lukavska, K. (2012). Time perspective as a predictor of massive multiplayer online role-playing game playing. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 15(1), 50–54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKillop, J., Anderson, E. J., Castelda, B. A., Mattson, R. E., & Donovick, P. J. (2006). Divergent validity of measures of cognitive distortions, impulsivity, and time perspective in pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 22(3), 339–354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503–507.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mcclure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(21), 5796.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., & Poline, J.-B. (2005). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage, 25(3), 653–660.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, N., Soane, E., Fenton-O'Creevy, M., & Willman, P. (2005). Personality and domain-specific risk taking. Journal of Risk Research, 8(2), 157–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nigro, G., & Cosenza, M. (2016). Living in the now: Decision-making and delay discounting in adolescent gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(4), 1191–1202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri, M., Koch, G., Salerno, S., Torriero, S., Gerfo, E. L., & Caltagirone, C. (2009). Representation of time intervals in the right posterior parietal cortex: Implications for a mental time line. Neuroimage, 46(4), 1173–1179.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, M. P., Hozack, N. E., Zauscher, B. E., Frank, L., Brown, G. G., McDowell, J., & Braff, D. L. (2002). Parietal dysfunction is associated with increased outcome-related decision-making in schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry, 51(12), 995–1004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, S. S., Pellecchia, G., Aminian, K., Ray, N., Segura, B., Obeso, I., & Strafella, A. P. (2013). Morphometric correlation of impulsivity in medial prefrontal cortex. Brain topography, 26(3), 479-487.

  • Pluck, G., Lee, K.-H., Lauder, H. E., Fox, J. M., Spence, S. A., & Parks, R. W. (2008). Time perspective, depression, and substance misuse among the homeless. The Journal of Psychology, 142(2), 159–168.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preuschoff, K., Quartz, S. R., & Bossaerts, P. (2008). Human insula activation reflects risk prediction errors as well as risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 2745–2752.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Przepiorka, A., & Blachnio, A. (2016). Time perspective in internet and Facebook addiction. Computers in Human Behavior, 60(C), 13–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Qin, J., & Han, S. (2009). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying identification of environmental risks. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 397–405.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F., & Lloyd, F. J. (2006). Physician decision making and cardiac risk: Effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 12(3), 179–195.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, R. N., & Bryan, A. (2004). Relationships between future orientation, impulsive sensation seeking, and risk behavior among adjudicated adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19(4), 428–445.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbloom, T. (2003). Risk evaluation and risky behavior of high and low sensation seekers. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 31(4), 375–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothspan, S., & Read, S. J. (1996). Present versus future time perspective and HIV risk among heterosexual college students. Health Psychology, 15(2), 131–134.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, G., Fong, G. T., Hall, P. A., Guignard, R., Beck, F., Mons, U., et al. (2013). Time perspective as a predictor of smoking status: Findings from the international tobacco control (ITC) surveys in Scotland, France, Germany, China, and Malaysia. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 346.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sekścińska, K., Rudzinska-Wojciechowska, J., & Maison, D. (2018). Individual differences in time perspectives and risky financial choices. Personality and Individual Differences, 120, 118–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sitkin, S. B., & Pablo, A. L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior. Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 9–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, B. W., Mitchell, D. G. V., Hardin, M. G., Jazbec, S., Fridberg, D., Blair, R. J. R., & Ernst, M. (2009). Neural substrates of reward magnitude, probability, and risk during a wheel of fortune decision-making task. Neuroimage, 44(2), 600–609.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Studer, B., Manes, F., Humphreys, G., Robbins, T. W., & Clark, L. (2013). Risk-sensitive decision-making in patients with posterior parietal and ventromedial prefrontal cortex injury. Cerebral Cortex, 25(1), 1–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Symmonds, M., Wright, N. D., Bach, D. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Deconstructing risk: Separable encoding of variance and skewness in the brain. Neuroimage, 58(4), 1139–1149.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tobler, P. N., O'Doherty, J. P., Dolan, R. J., & Schultz, W. (2007). Reward value coding distinct from risk attitude-related uncertainty coding in human reward systems. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97(2), 1621–1632.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in Prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk & Uncertainty, 5(4), 297–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valldeoriola, F., Ibarretxebilbao, N., Tolosa, E., Bargalló, N., Junqué, C., Zarei, M., & José Martí, M. (2010). Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson’s disease. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(6), 1162–1171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Ittersum, K. (2012). The effect of decision makers’ time perspective on intention–behavior consistency. Marketing Letters, 23(1), 263–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Leijenhorst, L., Crone, E. A., & Bunge, S. A. (2006). Neural correlates of developmental differences in risk estimation and feedback processing. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2158–2170.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venkatraman, V., Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., Luce, M. F., & Huettel, S. A. (2009). Separate neural mechanisms underlie choices and strategic preferences in risky decision making. Neuron, 62(4), 593–602.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Volz, K. G., Schubotz, R. I., & Cramon, D. Y. v. (2003). Predicting events of varying probability: Uncertainty investigated by fMRI. Neuroimage, 19(2), 271–280.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. U., & Milliman, R. A. (1997). Perceived risk attitudes: Relating risk perception to risky choice. Management Science, 43(2), 123–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wills, T. A., Sandy, J. M., & Yaeger, A. M. (2001). Time perspective and early-onset substance use: A model based on stress–coping theory. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 118–125.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M., Simmons, A. N., Flagan, T., Lane, S. D., Wackermann, J., & Paulus, M. P. (2011). Neural substrates of time perception and impulsivity. Brain Research, 1406, 43–58.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Worrell, F. C., & Mello, Z. R. (2007). The reliability and validity of Zimbardo time perspective inventory scores in academically talented adolescents. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 67(3), 487–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P., & Boyd, J. (2008). The time paradox: The new psychology of time that will change your life: Simon and Schuster.

  • Zimbardo, P. G., Keough, K. A., & Boyd, J. N. (1997). Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(6), 1007–1023.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31971026).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Ting Xu and Zhiyi Chen conducted and analyzed the pilot studies. Ting Xu, Rong Zhang and Yaqi Yang collected and analyzed the data. Ting Xu, Zhiyi Chen, Fuschia Sirois and Tingyong Feng prepared and revised the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tingyong Feng.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors (including Ms. Ting Xu, Mr. Zhiyi Chen, Dr. Fuschia Sirois, Ms. Rong Zhang, Ms. Yaqi Yang and Dr. Tingyong Feng) declare they have no conflict of interest.

Informed consent statement

All procedures followed were in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, and the applicable revisions at the time of the investigation. Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Xu, T., Chen, Z., Sirois, F.M. et al. Neuroanatomical substrates accounting for the effect of present hedonistic time perspective on risk preference: the mediating role of right posterior parietal cortex. Brain Imaging and Behavior 15, 244–254 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00251-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00251-z

Keywords

Navigation