Skip to main content
Log in

English phonological specificity predicts early French reading difficulty in emerging bilingual children

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of the present study was to examine the predictive value of a dynamic test of English and French lexical specificity on at-risk reading classification in 13 at-risk and 44 not at-risk emerging English (L1)–French (L2) bilingual Grade 1 children (M = 75.87 months, SD = 3.18) enrolled in an early French immersion program in Canada. Lexical specificity was assessed with a computerized word learning game in which children were taught new English (e.g., “foal” and “sole”) and French (e.g., bac “bin” and bague “ring”) word pairs contrasted by minimal phonological differences. The results indicated that the dynamic test of lexical specificity in English contributed significantly to the prediction of children’s French at-risk reading status at the end of Grade 1 after controlling for French phonological awareness and nonverbal reasoning skills. However, French lexical specificity did not predict children’s reading risk classification in French after controlling for French phonological awareness. Thus, it may be feasible to identify at-risk status in emerging bilinguals using dynamic measures in their stronger language.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. T-tests among ELL/EL1 (at-risk) and ELL/EL1 (not at-risk) children revealed equal variances among the groups based on Levene’s test for equality of variances. There were no significant differences in means among the groups on any of the measures reported in this study (available from the authors upon request).

  2. A higher threshold for the ceiling rule was implemented in French because results obtained from piloting the measure revealed that children required six (as opposed to three) items in order to be able to consider the remaining higher-level items as too difficult.

References

  • Au-Yeung, K., Hipfner-Boucher, K., Chen, X., Pasquarella, A., D'Angelo, N., & Deacon, H. (2015). Development of English and French language and literacy skills in EL1 and EL French immersion students in the early grades. Reading Research Quarterly, 50, 233–254. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridges, M. S., & Catts, H. W. (2011). The use of a dynamic screening of phonological awareness to predict risk for reading disabilities in kindergarten children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 330–338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219411407863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caffrey, E., Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (2008). The predictive validity of dynamic assessment: A review. The Journal of Special Education, 41, 254–270. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022466907310366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C., Bridges, M. S., & Mendoza, K. (2009). Floor effects associated with universal screening and their impact on the early identification of reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 163–176. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219408326219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H., Nielsen, D., Bridges, M., Liu, Y., & Bontempo, D. (2015). Early identification of reading disabilities within an RTI framework. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48, 281–297. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413498115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, A., & Slavin, R. (2013). Effects of educational technology applications on reading outcomes for struggling readers: A best-evidence synthesis. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(3), 277–299 Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/43497623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, E., Compton, D. L., Gilbert, J. K., Steacy, L. M., Collins, A. A., & Lindström, E. R. (2017). Development of first-graders’ word reading skills: For whom can dynamic assessment tell us more? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50, 95–112. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219415599343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, S. C., Chen, X., & Deacon, S. H. (2017). The relation between orthographic processing and spelling in grade 1 French immersion children. Journal of Research in Reading, 41, 290–311. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, S. C., Chen, X., & Geva, E. (2018). Deconstructing and reconstructing cross-language transfer in bilingual reading development: An interactive framework. Journal of Neurolinguistics. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.01.003.

  • Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, É., & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., & Bryant, J. D. (2006). Selecting at-risk readers in first grade for early intervention: A two-year longitudinal study of decision rules and procedures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 394–409. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49, 222–251. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543049002222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In California State Department of Education (Ed.), Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework (pp. 3–49). Los Angeles: National Dissemination and Assessment Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLong, E. R., DeLong, D. M., & Clarke-Pearson, D. L. (1988). Comparing the areas under two or more correlated receiver operating characteristic curves: A nonparametric approach. Biometrics, 837–845. https://doi.org/10.2307/2531595.

  • E-Prime (2012) (Version 2.0) [Computer software]. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology software tools.

  • Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Bouton, B., & Caffrey, E. (2011). The construct and predictive validity of a dynamic assessment of young children learning to read: Implications for RTI frameworks. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 339–347. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219411407864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gellert, A. S., & Elbro, C. (2017). Does a dynamic test of phonological awareness predict early reading difficulties?: A longitudinal study from kindergarten through grade 1. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50, 227–237. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219415609185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gellert, A. S., & Elbro, C. (2018). Predicting reading disabilities using dynamic assessment of decoding before and after the onset of reading instruction: A longitudinal study from kindergarten through grade 2. Annals of Dyslexia, 68, 126–144. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-018-0159-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genesee, F. (2007). French immersion and at-risk students: A review of research evidence. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 63(5), 655–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Ryan, E. B. (1993). Linguistic and cognitive correlates of academic skills in first and second languages. Language Learning, 43, 5–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00171.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Schuster, B. (2000). Understanding individual differences in word recognition skills and ESL children. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 123–154 Retrieved from: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11881-000-0020-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Massey-Garrison, A. (2013). A comparison of the language skills of ELLs and monolinguals who are poor decoders, poor comprehenders, or normal readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46, 387–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219412466651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Collins, P., Baciu, I., & Gebotys, R. (2008). Predictors of grade 2 word reading, listening comprehension and reading comprehension from grade 1 variables in Spanish-speaking children: Similarities and differences. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 23, 11–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00259.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Dynamic testing. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 75–111. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C. (2002). Phonemes, rimes, and the mechanisms of early reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 58–64. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2002.2674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., Cormier, P., Levy, B. A., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2013). Discrimination of English and French orthographic patterns by biliterate children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 469–488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, C., Segers, E., McQueen, J. M., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Lexical specificity training effects in second language learners. Language Learning, 65, 358–389. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, C., Segers, E., McQueen, J. M., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Transfer from implicit to explicit phonological abilities in first and second language learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 795–812. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R. K., & Swain, M. (Eds.). (1997). Immersion education: International perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (2008). Impacts of prior literacy experience on second language learning to read. In K. Koda & A. M. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages: Cross-linguistic relationships in first- and second-language literacy development (pp. 68–96). New York: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Krenca, K., Segers, E., Chen, X., Shakory, S., Steele, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2019). Phonological specificity relates to phonological awareness and reading ability in English-French bilingual children. Reading and Writing an Interdisciplinary Journal 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09959-2

  • Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2010). Beyond cross-language transfer: Reconceptualizing the impact of early bilingualism on phonological awareness. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14(4), 365–385. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888431003623470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lidz, C. S. (1997). Dynamic assessment: Psychoeducational assessment with cultural sensitivity. Journal of Social Distress and Homeless, 6, 95–111. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02938530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lidz, C. S., & Elliott, J. (Eds.). (2000). Dynamic assessment: Prevailing models and applications. New York: Jai.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W., Palma, M. D., Frijters, J., Steinbach, K., Temple, M., Benson, N., & Lacerenza, L. (2008). Interventions for reading difficulties: A comparison of response to intervention by ELL and EFL struggling readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 333–352. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219408317859.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lu, Y. Y., & Hu, C. F. (2018). Dynamic assessment of phonological awareness in young foreign language learners: Predictability and modifiability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 1–18 Retrieved from: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11145-018-9894-x.

  • MacCoubrey, S., Wade-Woolley, L., Klinger, D., & Kirby, J. (2004). Early identification of at- risk L2 readers. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 61, 11–29. https://doi.org/10.1353/cml.2004.0004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., & Lindsey, K. A. (2010). Cognitive and oral language contributors to reading disabilities in Spanish-English bilinguals. In A. Y. Durgunoglu & C. Goldenberg (Eds.), Language and literacy development in bilingual settings.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. H., Hulme, C., Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naglieri, J. A. (1985). Matrix Analogies Test—Short Form. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obadia, A. A., & Thériault, C. M. L. (1995). Attrition in French immersion programs: Possible solutions. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 506–529 Retrieved from: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED400674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Connor, R. E., & Jenkins, J. R. (1999). Prediction of reading disabilities in kindergarten and first grade. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 159–197. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0302_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ortéga, É., & Lété, B. (2010). eManulex: Electronic version of Manulex and Manulex-infra databases. Retrieved from http://www.manulex.org

  • Paradis, J., Emmerzael, K., & Duncan, T. S. (2010). Assessment of English language learners: Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43, 474–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.01.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Lam, K., Luo, Y. C., & Ramirez, G. (2011). Cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in Chinese–English bilinguals. Journal of Research in Reading, 34(1), 23–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01484.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, D., & Gillam, R. (2015). Predicting reading ability for bilingual Latino children using dynamic assessment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(1), 3–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413486930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, D. B., Allen, M. M., & Spencer, T. D. (2016). Predicting reading difficulty in first grade using dynamic assessment of decoding in early kindergarten: A large-scale longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49, 200–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219414538518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, D. B., Gragg, S. L., & Spencer, T. D. (2018). Predicting reading problems 6 years into the future: Dynamic assessment reduces bias and increases classification accuracy. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(4), 875–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(Pt 2), 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speece, D. L., & Case, L. P. (2001). Classification in context: An alternative approach to identifying early reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology93(4), 735–749. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.4.735

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin: PRO-ED Publishing, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Tzuriel, D. (2001). Dynamic assessment of young children. In Dynamic assessment of young children (pp. 63–75). Boston: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Van Goch, M. M., McQueen, J. M., & Verhoeven, L. (2014). Learning phonologically specific new words fosters rhyme awareness in Dutch preliterate children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 155–172. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.827199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Pearson, N. A. (2013). CTOPP-2: Comprehensive test of phonological processing (2nd ed.). Austin: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (1988). The MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine usable dictionary, Version 2.00. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20, 6–10. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, N., D’Angelo, N., & Chen, X. (2016). A school-based phonological awareness intervention for struggling readers in early French immersion. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 183–205 Retrieved from: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1089591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson tests of achievement. Itasca: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

A special thanks to Dr. Jeffrey Steele, Dr. Eliane Segers, Dr. Ludo Verhoeven, Sharry Shakory, and Alexandra Bellissimo for their invaluable guidance and support throughout the project. This research would not have been possible without the support of the teachers, parents, and children at our partner schools, in addition to all of the research assistants of the Multilingualism and Literacy Lab.

Funding

This research was funded by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Insight Development Grant No. 430-2015-00786 (to X.C.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Klaudia Krenca.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Krenca, K., Gottardo, A., Geva, E. et al. English phonological specificity predicts early French reading difficulty in emerging bilingual children. Ann. of Dyslexia 70, 27–42 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-019-00188-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-019-00188-4

Keywords

Navigation