Skip to main content
Log in

Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: the late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Theory in Biosciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Iconic representations (i.e., figurative imagery and realistic art) only started to appear consistently some 45,000 years ago, although humans have been anatomically modern since 200,000–160,000 years ago. What explains this? Some authors have suggested a neurocognitive change took place, leading to a creative explosion, although this has been contested. Here, we examine the hypothesis that demographic changes caused cultural “cumulative adaptive evolution” and as such the emergence of modern symbolic behavior. This approach usefully explains the evolution of utilitarian skills and tools, and the creation of symbols to identify groups. However, it does not equally effectively explain the evolution of behaviors that may not be directly adaptive, such as the production of iconic representations like figurines and rock art. In order to shed light on their emergence, we propose to combine the above-mentioned cultural hypothesis with the concept of sensory exploitation. The concept essentially states that behavioral traits (in this case iconic art production) which exploit pre-existing sensory sensitivities will evolve if not hindered by costs (i.e., natural selection). In this view, iconic art traditions are evolved by piggybacking on cumulative adaptive evolution. Since it is to date uncertain whether art has served any adaptive function in human evolution, parsimony demands paying more attention to the primary and afunctional mechanism of sensory exploitation as opposed to mechanisms of models based exclusively on secondary benefits (such as Miller’s, for instance, in which art is proposed to evolve as a sexual display of fitness).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term “psychosensory” is used here as a synonym for “sensory” to stress that we do not only focus on hidden preferences but also on adaptive sensory biases (see further)—which often have a learned and emotional aspect, and a psychological, social and even cultural dimension.

  2. For biologists the term “mimic” usually refers to a whole, mimicking organism (e.g., Pasteur 1982), but Maran (2007, p. 237)—in our opinion usefully—argues from the semioticist view: “…neither the mimic nor the model needs to be a whole organism but can be just a part of an organism both in spatial or temporal terms or just a perceptible feature.” Therefore, here we use mimic in the latter sense.

  3. In this case, the adaptive attention toward vital reproductively functional parts of her own body.

  4. Some also make a distinction between biophilia and biophobia: the former refers to positive, while the latter to negative affective states towards natural-like processes and elements (see Ulrich 1993). This, however, seems largely a terminological discussion. The crux of the matter is that there are some biologically based affective responses to biological categories.

  5. According to Boyd and Richerson (1985, p. 278) each culture may contain a number of afunctional or counterfunctional traits.

Abbreviations

SE:

Sensory exploitation

UP:

Upper Paleolithic

LSA:

Late Stone Age

References

  • Ambrose SH (1998a) Chronology of the Later Stone Age and food production in East Africa. J Archaeol Sci 25:377–392

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambrose SH (1998b) Late Pleistocene human population bottlenecks, volcanic winter, and differentiation of modern humans. J Hum Evol 34:623–651

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arak A, Enquist M (1993) Hidden preferences and the evolution of signals. Phil Trans R Soc B 340:207–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnqvist G (2006) Sensory exploitation and sexual conflict. Phil Trans R Soc B 361:375–386. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1790

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Yosef O (2002) The Upper Paleolithic revolution. Annu Rev Anthropol 31:363–393

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bednarik RG (2003) A figurine from the African Acheulian. Curr Anthropol 44(3):405–413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Brumm A, Moore MW (2005) Symbolic revolutions and the Australian archaeological record. Camb Archaeol J 15:157–175. doi:10.1017/S0959774305000089

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coe K (2003) The ancestress hypothesis: visual art as adaptation. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick

    Google Scholar 

  • Conard NJ (2003) Palaeolithic ivory sculptures from South-Western Germany and the origins of figurative art. Nature 426:830–832

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conard NJ (2009) A female figurine from the basal Aurignacian of Hohle Fels Cave in southwestern Germany. Nature 459:248–252. doi:10.1038/nature07995

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dart RA (1974) The waterworn pebble of many faces from Makapansgat. S Afr J Sci 70:167–169

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, 2 vols. John Murray, London (Reprinted in 1952 by Encyclopedia Britannica)

  • Debeaune SA, White R (1993) Ice-age lamps. Sci Am 268:108–113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delporte H (1977) Paleolithic lamp found at cave-of-Lascaux. Rev Louvre mus Fr 27:94–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake E (1992) Homo sestheticus. Where art comes from and why. University of Washington Press, Seattle

    Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake E (2001) Birth of the arts. Nat Hist 109:84–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler JA, Basolo AL (1998) Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection. Trends Ecol Evol 13:415–420

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Floss H, Rouquerol N (2007) Les Chemins de l’Art Aurignacien en Europe/Das Aurignacien und die Anfänge der Kunst in Europa. Éditions Musée-forum Aurignac, Aurignac

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller RC, Houle D, Travis J (2005) Sensory bias as an explanation for the evolution of mate preferences. Am Nat 166:437–446

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goren-Inbar N (1986) A figurine from the Acheulean site of Berekhat Ram. J Isr Prehist Soc 19:7–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford T, Dawkins MS (1991) Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim Behav 42:1–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses—the Tasmanian case. Am Antiq 69:197–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson D (2006) Understanding the origins of Paleoart: the neurovisual resonance theory and brain functioning. Paleoanthropology 2006:54–67

    Google Scholar 

  • Huffman MA, Quiatt D (1986) Stone handling by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): implications for tool use of stone. Primates 27:413–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James HVA, Petraglia MD (2005) Interpreting conflict in the ancient Andes. Curr Anthropol 46(Suppl):3. doi:10.1086/425660

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellert SR, Wilson EO (1993) The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein R (2000) Archaeology and the evolution of human behavior. Evol Anthropol 9:17–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn M, Mithen S (1999) Handaxes: products of sexual selection? Antiquity 73:518–526

    Google Scholar 

  • Kokko H, Brooks R, Jennions MD, Morley J (2003) The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proc R Soc Lond B 270:653–664

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maran T (2007) Semiotic interpretations of biological mimicry. Semiotica 167:223–248. doi:10.1515/SEM.2007.077

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marean CW et al (2007) Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene. Nature 449:905–908. doi:10.1038/nature06204

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty S, Brooks AS (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Hum Evol 39:453–563

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott L (1996) Self-representation in upper paleolithic female figurines. Curr Anthropol 37:227–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars P (2005) The impossible coincidence. A single-species model for the origins of modern human behavior in Europe. Evol Anthropol 14:12–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller GF (1998) How mate choice shaped human nature: a review of sexual selection and human evolution. In: Crawford C, Krebs D (eds) Handbook of evolutionary psychology: ideas, issues, and applications. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 87–129

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GF (1999) Sexual selection for cultural displays. In: Dunbar R, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 71–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GF (2000) The mating mind. Heinemann, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GF (2001) Aesthetic fitness: how sexual selection shaped artistic virtuosity as a fitness indicator and aesthetic preferences as mate choice criteria. Bull Psychol Arts 2(1):20–25

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen SJ (1996) The prehistory of the mind: a search for the origins of art, religion, and science. Thames and Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell JF, Allen J (2007) Pre-LGM Sahul (Pleistocene Australia-New Guinea) and the archaeology of early modern humans. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C (eds) Rethinking the human revolution. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, pp 395–410

    Google Scholar 

  • Pasteur G (1982) A classificatory review of mimicry systems. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 13:169–199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petraglia MD (2007) Mind the gap: factoring the Arabian Peninsula and the Indian subcontinent into out of Africa models. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C (eds) Rethinking the human revolution. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, pp 383–394

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (1997) How the mind works. Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (2002) The blank slate. The modern denial of human nature. Viking, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2009) Late pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:1298–1301. doi:10.1126/science.1170165

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Power C (1999) ‘Beauty’ magic: the origins of art. In: Dunbar R, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 71–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W (1999) The science of art: a neurological theory of aesthetic experience. J Conscious Stud 6:15–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Ribeiro PD, Christy JH, Rissanen RJ, Kim TW (2006) Males are attracted by their own courtship signals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:81–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2001) Built for speed, not for comfort. Darwinian theory and human culture. Hist Philos Life Sci 23:425–465

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross J, Davidson I (2006) Rock art and ritual: an archaeological analysis of rock art in arid central Australia. J Archaeol Method Theory 13:305–341. doi:10.1007/s10816-006-9021-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (1998) Sexual selection, receiver biases, and the evolution of sex differences. Science 281:1999–2003 (review 1999)

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salzburger W, Braasch I, Meyer A (2007) Adaptive sequence evolution in a color gene involved in the formation of the characteristic egg-dummies of male haplochromine cichlid fishes. BMC Biol 5:51

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebeok TA (1989) Iconicity. In: Sebeok TA (ed) The sign and its masters. University Press of America, Lanham, pp 107–127

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebeok TA, Danesi M (2000) The forms of meaning: modeling systems theory and semiotic analysis. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B (1992) Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. Brain 115:15–36

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan S (2001) Demography and cultural innovation: a model and its implication for the emergence of modern human culture. Camb Archeol J 11:5–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner MC, Kuhn SL (2006) Changes in the ‘Connectedness’ and resilience of Paleolithic societies in Mediterranean ecosystems. Hum Ecol 34:693–712

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ten Cate C, Rowe C (2007) Biases in signal evolution: learning makes a difference. Trends Ecol Evol 22:380–387. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2007.03.006

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tobler M (2006) Die Eiflecken bei Cichliden: Evolution durch Nutzung der Sinne? (The egg spots of cichlids: evolution through sensory exploitation?) Z Fisch 8:39–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsao DY, Livingstone MS (2008) Mechanisms of face perception. Annu Rev Neurosci 31:411–437. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094238

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich RS (1993) Biophilia, biophobia, and natural landscapes. In: Kellert RS, Wilson EO (eds) The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press, Washington, pp 73–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Damme W (2008) Introducing world art studies. In: Van Damme W, Zijlmans K (eds) World art studies: exploring concepts and approaches. Valiz, Amsterdam, pp 23–61

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik CP, Knott CD (2001) Geographic variation in tool use on Neesia fruits in orangutans. Am J Phys Anthropol 114:331–342

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verpooten J, Nelissen M (2010) Sensory exploitation: underestimated in the evolution of art as once in sexual selection? In: Plaisance KS, Reydon TAC (eds) Philosophy of behavioral biology, volume: Boston studies in the philosophy of science. Springer

  • Vogelsang R (1998) Middle-Stone-Age-Fundstellen in Südwest-Namibia. Heinrich Barth Institut, Köln

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachtel E (1993) The first picture show: cinematic aspects of cave art. Leonardo 26:135–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White R (1982) Rethinking the middle/upper paleolithic transition (and comments and replies). Curr Anthropol 23:169–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson EO (1984) Biophilia. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank one anonymous referee, Johan Braeckman and his research group, Andreas de Block, Tijs Goldschmidt, Yannick Joye, and Alexander Verpoorte for sharing useful suggestions and commenting on earlier drafts. Also many thanks to Lokaal01_Antwerpen (www.lokaal01.org) for the opportunity to discuss our ideas among scientists and artists. Last but not least we thank Nathalie Gontier and the other members of the organizing committee for having us at the conference.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan Verpooten.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Verpooten, J., Nelissen, M. Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: the late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution. Theory Biosci. 129, 211–221 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12064-010-0095-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12064-010-0095-7

Keywords

Navigation