Skip to main content
Log in

Attentional Effort, Mindfulness, and Altered States of Consciousness Experiences Following Quadrato Motor Training

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The scientific study of movement-related contemplative practices has proceeded without much attention to the range of psychological and phenomenological changes thought to occur during the practice. Quadrato Motor Training (QMT) is a specifically structured walking meditation, recently found to improve creativity and reflectivity, as well as neuroplasticity. This paper presents first-person reports related to QMT-induced experiences, derived from both practitioners of breathing meditation (BM) who practiced the QMT for 1 week (n = 15) compared to 4 weeks of daily training (n = 14) and control non-BM practitioners who practiced the QMT for 4 weeks of daily training (n = 14). Following factor analysis, the reported experiences were classified into three categories: Attentional Effort, Mindfulness, and Altered States of Consciousness (ASC). Our analysis revealed significant group differences, with increased ASC and attentional effort experiences reported by the groups that practiced the QMT for 4 weeks, but not in the group that practiced it for only 1 day. We further build on the previous QMT-induced electrophysiological and cognitive changes and the meditation literature to posit the possible underlying mechanisms of QMT-induced ASC experiences, in order to suggest a novel interpretation of QMT that calls attention to its structural similarities with meditation. By providing and contextualizing these reports of QMT-induced experiences, scientists, clinicians, and meditators can gain a more informed view of the range of experiences that can be elicited by whole-body contemplative practices.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aftanas, L. I., & Golosheikin, S. A. (2003). Changes in cortical activity in altered states of consciousness: the study of meditation by high-resolution EEG. Human Physiology, 29(2), 143–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. H. (1998). Zen and the brain. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment, 11(3), 191–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Soussan, T. D., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., Berkovich-Ohana, A., & Donchin, O. (2013). Into the square and out of the box: the effects of quadrato motor training on creativity and alpha coherence. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e55023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Soussan, T. D., Avirame, K., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., Harpaz, Y., & Ben-Shachar, M. (2014). Changes in cerebellar activity and inter-hemispheric coherence accompany improved reading performance following quadrato motor training. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Soussan, T. D., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Piervincenzi, C., Glicksohn, J., & Carducci, F. (2015). Embodied cognitive flexibility and neuroplasticity following quadrato motor training. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1021.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2013). Alterations in the sense of time, space and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: a neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(1), 1–19. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00912.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(27), 11483–11488.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. P., & Gerbarg, P. L. (2005). Sudarshan Kriya yogic breathing in the treatment of stress, anxiety, and depression: part I—neurophysiologic model. Journal of Alternative & Complementary Medicine, 11(1), 189–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. P., & Gerbarg, P. L. (2009). Yoga breathing, meditation, and longevity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1172(1), 54–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 180–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 303–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., Goleman, D. J., & Schwartz, G. E. (1976). Attentional and affective concomitants of meditation: a cross-sectional study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85(2), 235–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S., & Sheridan, J. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 564–570. doi:10.1097/01.PSY.0000077505.67574.E3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical representations of interoceptive attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nss, 066, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gard, T., Noggle, J. J., Park, C. L., Vago, D. R., & Wilson, A. (2014). Potential self-regulatory mechanisms of yoga for psychological health. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(1), 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gifford-May, D., & Thompson, N. (1994). "Deep states" of meditation: phenomenological reports of experience. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 26(2), 117–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glicksohn, J. (1993). Altered sensory environments, altered states of consciousness and altered-state cognition. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 14(1), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glicksohn, J. (2001). Temporal cognition and the phenomenology of time: a multiplicative function for apparent duration. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(1), 1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goleman, D. (1984). The Buddha on meditation and states of consciousness. In D. H. Shapiro (Ed.), Meditation. Classical and contemporary perspectives (pp. 317–360). New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goleman, D. (1988). The meditative mind—the varieties of meditative experience. New York: GP Putnam & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. NeuroImage, 59(1), 750–760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hölzel, B., & Ott, U. (2006). Relationships between meditation depth, absorption, meditation practice, and mindfulness: a latent variable approach. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 38(2), 179–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 537–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jahnke, R., Larkey, L., Rogers, C., Etnier, J., & Lin, F. (2010). A comprehensive review of health benefits of Qigong and Tai Chi. American Journal of Health Promotion, 24(6), e1–e25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1985). The varieties of religious experience. Boston: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, C. G., Vangkilde, S., Frokjaer, V., & Hasselbalch, S. G. (2012). Mindfulness training affects attention—or is it attentional effort? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 106–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, C. E., Sacchet, M. D., Lazar, S. W., Moore, C. I., & Jones, S. R. (2013). Mindfulness starts with the body: somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(12), 12. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00012.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kvale, S. (1996). InterViews—an introduction to qualitative research interviewing. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkey, L., Jahnke, R., Etnier, J., & Gonzalez, J. (2009). Meditative movement as a category of exercise: implications for research. Journal of Physical Activity & Health, 6(2), 230–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazar, S. W., Bush, G., Gollub, R. L., Fricchione, G. L., Khalsa, G., & Benson, H. (2000). Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. Neuroreport, 11(7), 1581–1585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindahl, J. R., Kaplan, C. T., Winget, E. M., & Britton, W. B. (2013). A phenomenology of meditation-induced light experiences: traditional Buddhist and neurobiological perspectives. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Rawlings, N. B., Francis, A. D., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2009). Mental training enhances attentional stability: neural and behavioral evidence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(42), 13418–13427.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7(1), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. (1964). Religions, values, and peak experiences. Columbus: Ohio State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paoletti, P. (2008). Crescere nell’eccellenza. Rome: Armando Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paoletti, P., & Selvaggio, A. (2011). Osservazione. First edition. Rome, Edizioni 3P.

  • Payne, P., & Crane-Godreau, M. A. (2013). Meditative movement for depression and anxiety. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4(1), 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesce, C., & Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2015). Cogito ergo sum’ or ‘ambulo ergo sum’? New perspectives in developmental exercise and cognition research, in exercise-cognition interaction: Neuroscience Perspectives. McMorris, T. Ed., Elsevier, New York, NY, USA.

  • Piron, H. (2001). The Meditation Depth Index (MEDI) and the Meditation Depth Questionnaire (MEDEQ). Journal for Meditation and Meditation Research, 1, 69–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raffone, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). The exploration of meditation in the neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, S. L., Oman, D., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Flinders, T. (2008). Cultivating mindfulness: effects on well‐being. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64(7), 840–862.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sood, A., & Jones, D. T. (2013). On mind wandering, attention, brain networks, and meditation. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing, 9(3), 136–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperduti, M., Martinelli, P., & Piolino, P. (2012). A neurocognitive model of meditation based on activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 269–276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y. Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(5), 222–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 17152–17156.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tart, C. T. (1972). Altered states of consciousness. Oxford: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E., & Varela, F. (2001). Radical embodiment: neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 418–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 296. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vaitl, D., Birbaumer, N., Gruzelier, J., Jamieson, G. A., Kotchoubey, B., Kübler, A., & Pütz, P. (2005). Psychobiology of altered states of consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 98–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. (1996). Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3, 330–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., & Shear, J. (1999). First-person methodologies: what, why, how. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(2–3), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venditti, S., Verdone, L., Pesce, C., Tocci, N., Caserta, M., & Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2014). Creating Well-being: increased creativity and proNGF decrease following Quadrato Motor Training. BioMed Research International, 275062, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, B. A. (1999). The Buddhist tradition of Samatha: methods for refining and examining consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(2–3), 175–187.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

This study was funded by grant CUP J87I10000960008 from the Italian Minister of Foreign Affairs, as part of the Italy-Israel R&D Cooperation Program.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tal Dotan Ben-Soussan.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in the current study were approved and in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional ethical committee of Bar-Ilan University.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ben-Soussan, T.D., Glicksohn, J. & Berkovich-Ohana, A. Attentional Effort, Mindfulness, and Altered States of Consciousness Experiences Following Quadrato Motor Training. Mindfulness 8, 59–67 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-015-0469-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-015-0469-5

Keywords

Navigation