Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Effect of Schooling on Mortality: New Evidence From 50,000 Swedish Twins

  • Published:
Demography

Abstract

By using historical data on about 50,000 twins born in Sweden during 1886–1958, we demonstrate a positive and statistically significant relationship between years of schooling and longevity. This relation remains almost unchanged when exploiting a twin fixed-effects design to control for the influence of genetics and shared family background. This result is robust to controlling for within-twin-pair differences in early-life health and cognitive ability, as proxied by birth weight and height, as well as to restricting the sample to MZ twins. The relationship is fairly constant over time but becomes weaker with age. Literally, our results suggest that compared with low levels of schooling (less than 10 years), high levels of schooling (at least 13 years of schooling) are associated with about three years longer life expectancy at age 60 for the considered birth cohorts. The real societal value of schooling may hence extend beyond pure labor market and economic growth returns. From a policy perspective, schooling may therefore be a vehicle for improving longevity and health, as well as equality along these dimensions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. More studies have used the twin fixed-effects design to study the effect of schooling on various health outcomes, such as self-reported health, smoking, and obesity (e.g., Amin et al. 2013, 2015a; Behrman et al. 2015; Fujiwara and Kawachi 2009; Lundborg 2013; Madsen et al. 2014; Webbink et al. 2010).

  2. Economists have recently considered the relationship between education and a range of nonmonetary outcomes, such as health, criminal behavior, marriage, and political participation (see Lochner 2011 for an overview). This recent evidence points to the importance of education for a much wider range of outcomes than those observed on the labor market and thus suggests that the value of educational investments in society may be greater than previously thought.

  3. Later empirical research has found limited support for this specific mechanism (Cutler and Lleras-Muney 2010).

  4. Other twin studies, however, have found no significant association (Bonjour et al. 2003; Miller et al. 2005; Petersen et al. 2009).

  5. We impose the upper limit of 1958 because data for more recent cohorts of twins were not available to us.

  6. Selection issues are much less serious for those born in 1926–1958: average life expectancy had increased, and most twins would have survived until 1972–1973, when the first survey was conducted for these cohorts.

  7. The register does not cover Swedes who emigrated from Sweden and who no longer hold a civil registration in Sweden, however. Because we classify individuals who do not have a death date by 2009 in our data as still alive, we risk wrongly classifying individuals who emigrated and died abroad as still being alive. We believe this to be of little concern in our analyses, however, given that the number of emigrants in the cohorts used in our analyses is small. Although we cannot observe emigration directly in our data, we can draw on external sources. Statistics Sweden calculated the number of Swedes living abroad in 2003 for different age groups between 0 and 80 years (Nilsson 2004). For those aged 45 to 80 in 2003 (i.e., born between 1923 and 1958), the number of emigrants ranged from 250 among the early cohorts to 1,500 among the later cohorts; that is, only a small fraction of each birth cohort had emigrated. This fraction in our sample is substantially smaller because inclusion in our sample is conditional on surviving to 1972 and living in Sweden in 1972. We checked the number of potential emigrants by investigating how many individuals in our sample who lack income records in the last year of our income data (i.e., in 2007) had not died by 2007. The income records cover every source of income, including pensions. If there are no data, it is likely that the person either is dead or has emigrated. With this procedure, we found that 0.72 % (348 individuals) have no income and no death date. We reran our main regressions excluding this group, but the results were basically unchanged (results available on request).

  8. The information in these registers is reported from the various educational institutions directly to Statistics Sweden. Using the Swedish Standard Classification of Education (Svensk Utbildningsnomenklatur, SUN 2000), we impute years of schooling as follows: 6 for old primary school (“folkskola”) (born before 1935), 7 for old primary school (born 1935 onward), 9 for (new) compulsory primary school, 10 for one year of high school, 11 for two years of high school, 12 for three years of high school, 13 for one year of university studies, 14 for two years of university studies, 15 for three years of university studies, 16 for four years of university studies, 17 for five years of university studies, 18 for a licentiate, and 20 for a PhD.

  9. To impute years of schooling from the information in the 1970 census, we use the SUN codes in the census, which are somewhat less detailed than the SUN 2000 codes used in the 1990 and 2007 registers, together with information from Statistics Sweden. We assign years of schooling as follows: 6 for old primary school (“folkskola”) (born before 1935), 7 for old primary school (born in 1935 and onward), 9 for (new) compulsory primary school, 11 or two years of high school, 12 for three years of high school, 14 for two or fewer years of university studies, 15 for three or more years of university studies, and 18 for a licentiate or a PhD.

  10. In the 1973 survey, education was coded in the same way as in the 1970 census, and we impute years of schooling in the same way. For the 1961 survey, we assign years of schooling for the majority of the sample based on the self-reports. We impute the following way: 6 for old primary school (“folkskola”) (born before 1935), 7 for old primary school (born 1935 and onward), 9 for (new) compulsory primary school, 9 for “realskola,” and 12 for secondary school. For three of the categories (“occupational school,” “other,” and “unknown”), we are unable to directly impute years of schooling. We calculate the average years of schooling associated with these three categories for the subsample of people for whom we have data on education from both the 1990 register and the survey in 1963. We then impute these averages for the individuals for whom the only source of education data is the 1963 survey. We use this imputation procedure for only 2 % of the sample.

  11. Our figures on the fraction of pairs who differ in schooling corresponds relatively well with figures from previous studies using a twin design to study the returns to education (see, e.g., Ashenfelter and Rouse 1998; Holmlund et al. 2011).

  12. We do not include cohorts born before 1911 because the 1970 census does not include educational information for cohorts born earlier than 1911—that is, for those who were 60 and older in 1970. For comparability, we therefore restrict the comparison to cohorts born in 1911–1931.

  13. An alternative would have been to run fixed-effects regressions on the probability of surviving until certain ages or to study 5- or 10-year survival. We prefer duration models in our main specification because they better exploit the variation in the data. For 10-year survival, for instance, two related twins who die within the same 10-year period but on different dates would not contribute to the identification of the estimates in a fixed-effects model. In our preferred duration model, however, any differences in the death dates contribute to the identification of the estimates. To shed more light on the magnitude of our estimates, however, we also exploit fixed-effects models.

  14. See Ridder and Tunali (1999) for a thorough exposition of the SPL model.

  15. One cannot rule out that IQ differences between twins are generated through earlier differences in schooling inputs, such as teachers. In fact, this is what two Swedish studies found when using the same data Sandewall et al. (2014) used (Carlsson et al. 2013; Meghir et al. 2013).

  16. In principle, this possibility is testable if a good instrument exists to generate random variation in schooling between members of twin pairs. To the best of our knowledge, there are no twin data containing such instruments.

  17. In contrast, a few studies found that parents reinforce endowment differences between children (see, e.g., Behrman et al. 1994).

  18. The opposite would be true, implying upward bias, if the twin with less schooling rebelled by behaving differently from the better-educated co-twin.

  19. For comprehensive overviews of common criticisms to twin studies and the circumstances under which the estimated results are unbiased, see Amin et al. (2015b), Boardman and Fletcher (2015), and Kohler et al. (2011).

  20. We tested for statistically significant differences in the coefficient of schooling between MZ and DZ twins by pooling the samples and including an interaction term between schooling and being a MZ twin. This interaction term was insignificant, however (p = .34).

  21. The similarity in results with and without including twin fixed effects does not mean that genes are not important determinants of schooling or mortality. Instead, it suggests that the genes affecting schooling are not the same as those affecting mortality.

  22. However, an often mentioned potential advantage of within-twins estimates is that they might have less of a LATE flavor than IV estimates that depend on changing minimum schooling or school-leaving age requirements. The reason is that the twin fixed-effects estimates use variation in schooling over the entire schooling distribution, whereas many IV studies only use limited variation in schooling induced by the instrument (Amin et al. 2015b; Behrman et al. 2011).

  23. We also checked whether the twin pairs that differed in schooling also differed from other twin pairs in terms of observable characteristics. The results suggested that differences within twin pairs have become more common over time, as the average birth year of twin pairs that differed in schooling was 1937 compared with 1927 for twin pairs who did not differ. This difference was statistically significant. We return to the issue of changes in the estimates over cohorts/ages in subsequent sections.

  24. We also ran regressions excluding people who survived the ages of 100 and 90, respectively. We lost about 3,000 individuals by imposing this restriction, but the results barely changed.

  25. When we pool genders and interact an indicator for being male with schooling, the interaction term is insignificant in the SPL model (p = .68).

  26. The differences are not statistically significant, as revealed by insignificant interaction terms in both the SPL and in the standard Cox model (p = .68 and p = .17, respectively).

  27. We also tried alternative specifications of the birth weight variable, such as an indicator of low birth weight (birth weight <2,500 g). The indicator of low birth weight is significant, but when we included both this indicator and the birth weight variable, the latter always dominated and the former became insignificant.

  28. This would be true as long as at least some part of the variation in such preference traits is due to genetics. This is also what the studies by Cesarini et al. (2009) and Jang et al. (1996) suggest. A difference in results between MZ and DZ twins could also be explained by the greater role that measurement errors play among MZ twins than DZ twins. However, the bias resulting from relying on purely self-reported measures for education in estimating the association between schooling and health has been found to be of limited importance (e.g., Amin et al. 2015a, b). In addition, because data on education in this study are taken from registers for a large part of our sample, we do not believe that measurement errors play a great role.

  29. We also repeated the sensitivity tests of the previous sections, controlling for birth weight and height. The results were again robust.

  30. We cannot rule out that a strong downward bias from selection is offset by a strong upward bias from unobserved ability in the older cohorts. In later cohorts, a weaker selection bias would then also be offset by a weaker ability bias. It appears likely to us that improved access to education over time leads to less rationing of education according to skills. Moreover, when credit constraints are less binding, parents could more easily afford to send both twins to higher education institutions, whereas more binding constraints may lead parents to invest more in the more able twin. We can shed some light on this possibility by examining if the effect of height, which partly picks up cognitive ability, on education decreases over time. This is not the case, however; if anything, the effect is greater for the post-1940 birth cohorts.

  31. We tested this by including interaction terms between schooling and birth cohort indicators in the SPL model. For cohorts born in 1946–1958, the interaction term was significant for males and females combined (p < .01) and for males (p < .01), but was not significant for females (p = .36).

  32. The interaction terms between schooling and the cohort indicators for the age groups 60–70 and 70–80 were insignificant (p = .63 and p = .91, respectively)

  33. We test the proportional hazards assumption using Schoenfeld residuals (see the phtest option in Stata). For the age groups 50–60, 60–70, and 70–80, the Prob > χ2 are 0.55, 0.93, and 0.51, respectively.

  34. The Gompertz distribution has been shown to describe old age mortality rather well. This model is a parametric proportional hazard model in which the baseline hazard takes the following form:

    $$ {\uptheta}_0\left(t,a,b\right)=a{e}^{t/b}, $$
    (3)

    where a and e are shape and scale parameters, respectively, of the baseline; and t is, just as in the Cox and SPL models, the randomly distributed death time. Hence, the model can be expressed in terms similar to the Cox model with the addition that the baseline is specified according to a Gompertz distribution with specified shape and scale parameters.

  35. The main purpose of this exercise is not to exactly capture specific remaining life expectancies for different subgroups but rather to illustrate the differences in these expectancies—that is, to illustrate how the impact of education on mortality translates to longevity.

  36. On a computational level, the studied time/age profile is rescaled so that entrance in the relevant risk set occurs at time (t) 0 (at age 60) in order to more accurately capture old age mortality. The resulting estimates of the shape and scale parameters are then used to calculate life expectancies by integrating the random variable t with respect to its probability measure given by the estimated Gompertz density, over its support (0, inf)—that is, from age 60 (t = 0) onward.

  37. Around the millennium, the overall difference in life expectancy between the two countries was about three years, which has been attributed to a more unhealthy Danish lifestyle, especially in terms of alcohol and tobacco consumption (Juel 2008). Depending on its distribution within the population, this lifestyle may well interfere with the potential channels through which education may affect longevity.

  38. When we restrict our sample to the period 1921–1950 (the cohort studied by Behrman et al. (2011)) and run our SPL model on MZ and DZ twins, our estimate does not change much (0.950) and remains statistically significant.

  39. Another difference is that we are able to follow individuals until death in many cases, whereas they followed up until late adulthood. However, this difference seems to play a less important role because we find that the relationship between education and mortality becomes weaker with age rather than stronger.

References

  • Adams, P., Hurd, M. D., McFadden, D., Merrill, A., & Ribeiro, T. (2003). Healthy, wealthy, and wise? Tests for direct causal paths between health and socioeconomic status. Journal of Econometrics, 112, 3–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ahlstrom, K., Emanuelsson, I., & Wallin, E. (1986). Skolans krav - elevernas behov [School demands - student needs]. Lund, Sweden: Studentlitteratur.

  • Albouy, V., & Lequien, L. (2009). Does compulsory education lower mortality? Journal of Health Economics, 28, 155–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Almond, D., & Currie, J. (2011). Human capital development before age five. In D. Card & O. Ashenfelter (Eds.), Handbook of labor economics (Vol. 4B, pp. 1314–1486). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North Holland.

  • Amin, V. (2011). Returns to education: Evidence from UK twins: Comment. American Economic Review, 101, 1629–1635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amin, V., Behrman, J. R., & Kohler, H.-P. (2015a). Schooling has smaller or insignificant effects on adult health in the US than suggested by cross-sectional associations: New estimates using relatively large samples of identical twins. Social Science & Medicine, 127, 181–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amin, V., Behrman, J. R., Kohler, H.-P., Xiong, Y., & Zhang, J. (2015b). Causal inferences: Identical twins help and clarity about necessary assumptions is critical. Social Science & Medicine, 127, 201–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amin, V., Behrman, J., & Spector, T. D. (2013). Does more schooling improve health behaviours and health outcomes? Evidence from U.K. twins. Economics of Education Review, 35, 134–148.

  • Ashenfelter, O., & Rouse, C. (1998). Income, schooling and ability: Evidence from a new sample of identical twins. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 113, 253–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batty, G. D., Deary, I. J., & Gottfredson, L. S. (2007). Premorbid (early life) IQ and later mortality risk: Systematic review. Annals of Epidemiology, 17, 278–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, J. P., Cesarini, D., Johannesson, M., Lindqvist, E., & Apicella, C. (2011). On the sources of the height-intelligence correlation: New insights from a bivariate ACE model with assortative mating. Behavior Genetics, 41, 242–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behrman, J. R., Kohler, H.-P., Jensen, V. M., Pedersen, D., Petersen, I., Bingley, P., & Christensen, K. (2011). Does more schooling reduce hospitalization and delay mortality? New evidence based on Danish twins. Demography, 48, 1347–1375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behrman, J. R., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (2004). Returns to birthweight. Review of Economics and Statistics, 86, 586–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behrman, J. R., Rosenzweig, M. R., & Taubman, P. (1994). Endowments and the allocation of schooling in the family and in the marriage market: The twins experiment. Journal of Political Economy, 102, 1131–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behrman, J. R., Xiong, Y., & Zhang, J. (2015). Cross-sectional schooling-health associations misrepresented causal schooling effects on adult health and health-related behaviors: Evidence from the Chinese Adults Twins Survey. Social Science & Medicine, 127, 190–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, S. E., Devereux, P. J., & Salvanes, K. G. (2007). From the cradle to the labor market? The effect of birth weight on adult outcomes. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 409–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boardman, J. D., & Fletcher, J. M. (2015). To cause or not to cause? That is the question, but identical twins might not have all of the answers. Social Science & Medicine, 127, 198–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonjour, D., Cherkas, L., Haskel, J., Hawkes, D., & Spector, T. (2003). Returns to education: Evidence from UK twins. American Economic Review, 93, 1799–1812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bound, J., & Solon, G. (1999). Double trouble: On the value of twin-based estimation of the return to schooling. Economics of Education Review, 18, 169–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsson, M., Dahl, G. B., Ockert, B., & Rooth, D.-O. (2013). The effect of schooling on cognitive skills. Review of Economics and Statistics, 97, 533–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, A., & Paxson, C. (2008). Stature and status: Height, ability, and labor market outcomes. Journal of Political Economy, 116, 499–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Johannesson, M., Lichtenstein, P., & Wallace, B. (2009). Genetic variation in preferences for giving and risk-taking. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124, 809–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D., & Royer, H. (2012). The effect of education on adult mortality and health: Evidence from Britain. American Economic Review, 103, 2087–2120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. (2007). The technology of skill formation. American Economic Review, 97, 31–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currie, J. (2009). Healthy, wealthy, and wise? Socioeconomic status, poor health in childhood, and human capital development. Journal of Economic Literature, 47, 87–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currie, J., Stabile, M. P., & Roos, L. (2010). Child health and young adult outcomes. Journal of Human Resources, 45, 517–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, D., Deaton, A., & Lleras-Muney, A. (2006). The determinants of mortality. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20, 97–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, D. M., & Lleras-Muney, A. (2010). Understanding differences in health behaviors by education. Journal of Health Economics, 29, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, D. M., & Lleras-Muney, A. (2012). Education and health: Insights from international comparisons (NBER Working Paper No. 17738). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.

  • Fischer, M., Karlsson, M., & Nilsson, T. (2013). Effects of compulsory schooling on mortality: Evidence from Sweden. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10, 3596–3618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, V. R. (1982). Time preference and health: An exploratory study. In V. R. Fuchs (Ed.), Economic aspects of health (pp. 93–120). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fujiwara, T., & Kawachi, I. (2009). Is education causally related to better health? A twin fixed-effect study in the USA. International Journal of Epidemiology, 38, 1310–1322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathmann, C., Jtirges, H., & Reinhold, S. (2015). Compulsory schooling reforms, education and mortality in twentieth century Europe. Social Science & Medicine, 127, 74–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerdtham, U.-G., Lundborg, P., Lyttkens, C. H., & Nystedt, P. (2016). Do education and income really explain inequalities in health? Applying a twin design. Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 118, 25–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glied, S., & Lleras-Muney, A. (2008). Technological innovation and inequality in health. Demography, 45, 741–761.

  • Grambsch, P. M., & Therneau, T. M. (1994). Proportional hazards tests and diagnostics based on weighted residuals. Biometrika, 81, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M. (1972). On the concept of health capital and the demand for health. Journal of Political Economy, 80, 223–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hederos Ericksson, K., Jantti, M., Lindahl, L., & Torssander, J. (2014). Trends in life expectancy by income and the role of specific causes of death (SOFI Working Paper No. 8/2014). Stockholm: Swedish Institute for Social Research.

  • Holmlund, H., Lindahl, M., & Plug, E. (2011). The causal effect of parent’s schooling on children’s schooling: A comparison of estimation methods. Journal of Economic Literature, 49, 615–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holsinger, T., Helms, M., & Plassman, B. (2007). Intelligence in early adulthood and life span up to 65 years later in male elderly twins. Age and Ageing, 36, 286–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vemon, P. A. (1996). Heritability of the Big Five personality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. Journal of Personality, 64, 577–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jayachandran, S., & Lleras-Muney, A. (2009). Life expectancy and human capital investments: Evidence from maternal mortality declines. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124, 349–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, K. (2008). Middellevetid og dpdelighed i Danmark sammenlignet med i Sverige: Hvad betyder rygning og alkohol? [Life expectancy and mortality in Denmark compared to Sweden. What is the effect of smoking and alcohol?]. Ugeskrift for Laeger, 170, 2423–2427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, M. C., Garver-Apgar, C. E., Wright, M. J., Martin, N. G., Corley, R. P., Stallings, M. C., . . . Zietsch, B. P. (2013). The genetic correlation between height and IQ: Shared genes or assortative mating? PLoS Genetics, 9(4). doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1003451

  • Kenkel, D. (1991). Health behavior, health knowledge and schooling. Journal of Political Economy, 99, 287–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, H.-P., Behrman, J. R., & Schnittker, J. (2011). Social science methods for twins data: Integrating causality, endowments and heritability. Journal of Biodemography and Social Biology, 57, 88–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunst, A. E., Bos, V., Lahelma, E., Bartley, M., Lissau, I., Regidor, E., . . . Mackenbach, J. P. (2005). Trends in socioeconomic inequalities in self-assessed health in 10 European countries. International Journal of Epidemiology, 34, 295–305.

  • Lager, A., Bremberg, S., & Vagero, D. (2009). The association of early IQ and education with mortality: 65 year longitudinal study in Malmo, Sweden. BMJ, 339, 52–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lager, A., & Torssander, J. (2012). Causal effect of education on mortality in a quasi-experiment on 1.2 million Swedes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 8461–8466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, P., De Faire, U., Floderus, B., Svartengren, M., Svedberg, P., & Pedersen, N. L. (2002). The Swedish 2007-04-27 21 Twin Registry: A unique resource for clinical, epidemiological and genetic studies. Journal of Internal Medicine, 252, 184–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lleras-Muney, A. (2005). The relationship between education and adult mortality. Review of Economic Studies, 72, 189–221.

  • Lochner, L. (2011). Nonproduction benefits of education: Crime, health, and good citizenship. In E. A. Hanushek, S. Machin, & L. Woessmann (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education (Vol. 4, pp. 183–282). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier Science.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lundborg, P. (2013). The health returns to education—What can we learn from twins? Journal of Population Economics, 26, 673–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundborg, P., Nilsson, A., & Rooth, D. O. (2011). Early life health and adult earnings: Evidence from a large sample of siblings and twins (IZA Discussion Paper No. 5804). Bonn, Germany: Institute for the Study of Labor.

  • Lundborg, P., Nystedt, P., & Rooth, D. (2014). Height and earnings: The role of cognitive and non-cognitive skills. Journal of Human Resources, 49, 141–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackenbach, J. P., Kulhanova, I., Bopp, M., Deboosere, P., Eikemo, T. A., Hoffmann, R., . . . Lundberg, O. (2015). Variations in the relation between education and cause-specific mortality in 19 European populations: A test of the “fundamental causes” theory of social inequalities in health. Social Science & Medicine, 127, 51–62.

  • Madsen, M., Andersen, P. K., Gerster, M., Andersen, A. M. N., Christensen, K., & Osler, M. (2014). Are the educational differences in incidence of cardiovascular disease explained by underlying familial factors? A twin study. Social Science & Medicine, 118, 182–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. K., Hummer, R. A., & Powers, D. A. (2012). Educational differences in U.S. adult mortality: A cohort perspective. American Sociological Review, 77, 548–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazumder, B. (2008). Does education improve health? A reexamination of the evidence from compulsory schooling laws. Economic Perspectives, 32, 2–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meghir, C., Palme, M., & Simeonova, E. (2013). Education, cognition and health: Evidence from a social experiment (NBER Working Paper No. 19002). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.

  • Miech, R., Pampel, F., Kim, J., & Rogers, R. G. (2011). The enduring association between education and mortality: The role of widening and narrowing disparities. American Sociological Review, 76, 913–934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P., Mulvey, C., & Martin, N. (2005). Birth weight and schooling and earnings: Estimates from a sample of twins. Economics Letters, 86, 387–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2003). Education, social status, and health. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

  • Montez, J. K., & Friedman, E. M. (2015). Educational attainment and adult health: Under what conditions is the association causal? Social Science & Medicine, 127, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson, A. (2004). Immigration and emigration in the postwar period (Demographic Reports 2004:5). Stockholm: Statistics Sweden.

  • Oreopoulos, P., Stabile, M., Walld, R., & Roos, L. L. (2008). Short-, medium-, and long-term consequences of poor infant health: An analysis using siblings and twins. Journal of Human Resources, 43, 88–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Persico, N., Postlewaite, A., & Silverman, D. (2004). The effect of adolescent experience on labor market outcomes: The case of height. Journal of Political Economy, 112, 1019–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, I., Jensen, V. M., McGue, M., Bingley, P., & Christensen, K. (2009). No evidence of genetic mediation in the association between birthweight and academic performance in 2,413 Danish adolescent twin pairs. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 564–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridder, G., & Tunali, I. (1999). Stratified partial likelihood estimation. Journal of Econometrics, 92, 193–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, M. R., & Schultz, T. P. (1982). The behavior of mothers as inputs to child health: The determinants of birth weight, gestation, and rate of fetal growth. In V. R. Fuchs (Ed.), Economic aspects of health (pp. 53–92). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press for the National Bureau of Economic Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, H. (2009). Separated at girth: US twin estimates of the effects of birth weight. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 1, 49–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandewall, O., Cesarini, D., & Johannesson, M. (2014). Co-twin methodology and the return to schooling—Testing a critical assumption. Labour Economics, 26, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Sweden. (2014). Statistical yearbook of Sweden. Orebro: Statistics Sweden, Communication Department.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, D. H., Jr., Hasselblad, V., Henley, S. J., Thun, M. J., & Sloan, F. A. (2002). Benefits of smoking cessation for longevity. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 990–996.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torssander, J., & Erikson, R. (2010). Stratification and mortality—A comparison of education, class, status, and income. European Sociological Review, 26, 465–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Kippersluis, H., O’Donnell, O., & van Doorslaer, E. (2011). Long run returns to education: Does schooling lead to an extended old age? Journal of Human Resources, 46, 695–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webbink, D., Martin, N. G., & Visscher, P. M. (2010). Does education reduce the probability of being overweight? Journal of Health Economics, 29, 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, R. A., Ashby, V. B., Milford, E. L., Ojo, A. O., Ettenger, R. E., Agodoa, L. Y., . . . Port, F. K. (1999). Comparison of mortality in all patients on dialysis, patients on dialysis awaiting transplantation, and recipients of a first cadaveric transplant. New England Journal of Medicine, 341, 1725–1730.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank participants at the 22nd European Workshop on Econometrics and Health Economics and at the 23rd EALE conference for useful comments. We are also grateful for comments by seminar participants at Lund University and Linneaus University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Petter Lundborg.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lundborg, P., Lyttkens, C.H. & Nystedt, P. The Effect of Schooling on Mortality: New Evidence From 50,000 Swedish Twins. Demography 53, 1135–1168 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-016-0489-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-016-0489-3

Keywords

Navigation