Skip to main content
Log in

Changing Racial Bias by Transfer of Functions in Equivalence Classes

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Several studies used the stimulus equivalence paradigm to investigate attitudes toward socially relevant stimuli. In one of these studies, de Carvalho and de Rose (The Psychological Record 64: 527–536, 2014) used matching-to-sample (MTS) training to establish equivalence relations between a positive symbol and faces of individuals of African descent (toward which children showed negative bias prior to the research). Only one of four children showed the intended classes. The present study manipulated training parameters to increase the yield of equivalence classes comprising relations contrary to children’s previous racial bias. Thirteen children learned matching tasks that would potentially establish equivalence relations between Black faces and positive symbols, contrary to their preexperimental bias. All 13 children showed equivalence class formation, and nine of them maintained relations between Black faces and positive symbols in a different and more stringent test. Children’s evaluations of the faces with the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) showed a pronounced negative bias toward Black faces before training. After class formation, however, the difference between evaluations of Black and White faces was no longer statistically significant. These results showed that procedures based on equivalence and transfer of functions may contribute to educational programs designed to decrease racial biases, a significant challenge for our increasingly multicultural and multiracial societies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The terms negro (Black) and branco (White) are generally used in Brazil to refer to races, both by the individuals themselves and by official agencies, with no opprobrium. Because the research was done in Brazil, we will use the closest translation of the terms used in the country throughout this paper.

  2. A simpler training could just reverse the presumed preexperimental relations, in order to establish classes comprised by A1/B1/C1 and A2/B2/C3 (i.e., Black faces equivalent to the positive symbol and White faces equivalent to the negative symbol). We considered (as did de Carvalho & de Rose, 2014) it ethically unacceptable to expose children to a training that could establish equivalence relations between racial traits and a negative sign. Therefore, we used the same strategy as de Carvalho and de Rose (i.e., providing training that could establish equivalence relations between Black faces and a positive sign, and between a negative sign and an abstract symbol).

  3. These images were provided by the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley & Cuthbert, 1999). The picture selection was based on a study (McManis, Bradley, Berg, Cuthbert & Lang, 2001) that verified which pictures children aged between 7 and 11 evaluated as positive, negative, and neutral according to SAM results. We used only one image that was not used in McManis et al. study (rotten fruits), which is, nevertheless, present in the IAPS.

References

  • Adams, B. J., Fields, L., & Verhave, T. (1993). Effects of test order on intersubject variability during equivalence class formation. The Psychological Record, 43, 133–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amd, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Ivanoff, J. (2013). A derived transfer of eliciting emotional functions using differences among electroencephalograms as a dependent measure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99, 318–334. doi:10.1002/jeab.19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arntzen, E. (2006). Delayed matching to sample: probability of responding in accord with equivalence as a function of different delays. The Psychological Record, 56, 135–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arntzen, E., Fagerstrom, A., & Foxall, G. R. (2016). Equivalence classes and preferences in consumer choice. In G. R. Foxall (Ed.), The Routledge companion to consumer behavior analysis (pp. 65–77). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32, 169–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Keane, J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). A derived transfer of emotive functions as a means of establishing differential preferences for soft drinks. The Psychological Record, 50, 493–511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, A., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2010). The implicit relational assessment procedure: exploring the impact of private versus public contexts and the response latency criterion on pro-white and anti-black stereotyping among white Irish individuals. The Psychological Record, 60, 57–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortoloti, R., & de Rose, J. C. (2009). Assessment of the relatedness of equivalent stimuli through a semantic differential. The Psychological Record, 59, 563–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortoloti, R., & de Rose, J. C. (2011a). Avaliação do efeito de dica semântica e da indução de significado entre estímulos abstratos equivalentes [Evaluation of semantic priming effect and induction of meaning among abstract equivalent stimuli]. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 24, 381–393.

  • Bortoloti, R., & de Rose, J. C. (2011b). An “Orwellian” account of stimulus equivalence: are some stimuli “more equivalent” than others? European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 12, 121–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortoloti, R., & de Rose, J. C. (2012). Equivalent stimuli are more strongly related after training with delayed matching than after simultaneous matching: a study using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP). The Psychological Record, 62, 41–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortoloti, R., Rodrigues, N. C., Cortez, M. D., Pimentel, N., & de Rose, J. C. (2013). Overtraining increases the strength of equivalence relations. Psychology & Neuroscience, 6, 357–364. doi:10.3922/j.psns.2013.3.13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59.

  • Bush, K. M., Sidman, M., & de Rose, T. (1989). Contextual control of emergent equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 29–45. doi:10.1901/jeab.1989.51-29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, D., Wilkinson, K. M., Blackman, D., & McIlvane, W. J. (2000). Equivalence classes in individuals with minimal verbal repertoires. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 101–114. doi:10.1901/jeab.2000.74-101.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Almeida, J. H., & de Rose, J. C. (2015). Changing the meaningfulness of abstract stimuli by the reorganization of equivalence classes: effects of delayed matching. The Psychological Record, 65, 451–461. doi:10.1007/s40732-015-0120-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Carvalho, M. P., & de Rose, J. C. (2014). Understanding racial attitudes through the stimulus equivalence paradigm. The Psychological Record, 64, 527–536. doi:10.1007/s40732-014-0049-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Rose, J. C., McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., Galpin, V. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1988). Emergent simple discrimination established by indirect relation to differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 1–20. doi:10.1901/jeab.1988.50-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257. doi:10.1901/jeab.1986.46-243.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., Rehfeldt, R. A., Zlomke, K. R., & Robinson, A. (2006). Exploring the development and dismantling of equivalence classes involving terrorist stimuli. The Psychological Record, 56, 83–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doran, E., & Fields, L. (2012). All stimuli are equal, but some are more equal than others: measuring relational preferences within an equivalence class. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 98, 243–256. doi:10.1901/jeab.2012.98-243.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dougher, M. J., Augustson, E., Markham, M. R., Greenway, D. E., & Wulfert, E. (1994). The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 331–351. doi:10.1901/jeab.1994.62-331.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Drake, C. E., Kellum, K. K., Wilson, K. G., Luoma, J. G., Weinstein, H. H., & Adams, C. H. (2010). Examining the implicit relational assessment procedure: four preliminary studies. The Psychological Record, 60, 81–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., & Hiris, J. (1997). Match to Sample Program (Version 11.1.3) [Computer software]. Waltham: E. K. Shriver Center for Mental Retardation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1994). A transfer of self-discrimination response functions through equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 251–267. doi:10.1901/jeab.1994.62-251.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Landon-Jimenez, D. V., Buffington, D. M., & Adams, B. J. (1995). Maintained nodal distance effects after equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 129–145. doi:10.1901/jeab.1995.64-129.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guerin, B. (2005). Combating everyday racial discrimination without assuming racists or racism: new intervention ideas from a contextual analysis. Behavior and Social Issues, 14, 46–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haydu, V. B., Camargo, J., & Bayer, H. (2015). Effects of preexperimental history on the formation of stimulus equivalence classes: a study with supporters of Brazilian soccer clubs. Psychology & Neuroscience, 8, 385–396. doi:10.1037/h0101276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 119–137. doi:10.1901/jeab.1991.56-119.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, J. C., Tierney, K. J., Robinson, C. P., Keenan, M., Watt, A., & Barnes, D. (1993). Differences between clinically anxious and non-anxious subjects in a stimulus equivalence training task involving threat words. The Psychological Record, 43, 153–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, M. E., Luoma, J. B., Vilardaga, R., Lilis, J., Nobles, R., & Hayes, S. C. (2016). Examining the role of psychological inflexibility, perspective taking, and empathic concern in generalized prejudice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46, 180–191. doi:10.1111/jasp.12355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McManis, M. H., Bradley, M. M., Berg, W. K., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotional reactions in children: verbal, physiological, and behavioral responses to affective pictures. Psychophysiology, 38, 222–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moss-Lourenco, P., & Fields, L. (2011). Nodal structure and stimulus relatedness in equivalence classes: post-class formation preference tests. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 343–368. doi:10.1901/jeab.2011.95-343.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moxon, P., Keenan, M., & Hine, L. (1993). Gender-role stereotyping and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 381–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabelo, L. Z., Bortoloti, R., & Souza, D. H. (2014). Dolls are for girls and not for boys: evaluating the appropriateness of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure for school-age children. The Psychological Record, 64(1), 71–77. doi:10.1007/s40732-014-0006-2.

  • Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: a research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: an expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22. doi:10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Silveira, M. V., Aggio, N. M., Cortez, M. D., Bortoloti, R., Rico, V. V., & de Rose, J. C. (2015). Maintenance of equivalence classes and transfer of functions: the role of the nature of stimuli. The Psychological Record. doi:10.1007/s40732-015-0152-1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003). Children’s emergent preferences for soft drinks: stimulus equivalence and transfer. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 603–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Straatman, G., Almeida, S. S., & de Rose, J. C. (2014). Computerized assessment of food preferences in adolescents undergoing the stimulus equivalence paradigm. Temas em Psicologia, 22, 613–624. doi:10.9788/TP2014.3-07.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, B. W. (2003). Match to Sample Program III [Computer software]. Worcester: UMass/Eunice Kennedy Shriver Center's Behavioral Sciences Department.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weatherly, J. N., Miller, K., & McDonald, T. W. (1999). Social influence as stimulus control. Behavior and Social Issues, 9, 25–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 125–144. doi:10.1901/jeab.1988.50-125.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Deisy de Souza, leader of INCT-ECCE, for her encouragement and support for this research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julio C. de Rose.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was funded by the Brazilian National Research Council (CNPq, Grant 573972/2008-7) and the São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP, Grant 2008/57705-8).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This research was approved by the institutional Ethical Review Board, in accordance with Brazilian guidelines for ethical conduct in research with human participants.

Additional information

Táhcita Mizael and Carolina Silveira were supported by Graduate fellowships from the Brazilian Ministry of Education (CAPES). João de Almeida was supported by a post-doctoral fellowship from the São Paulo Research Foundation (Grant # 2014/01874-7). Julio de Rose was supported by a Research Productivity Grant from the National Research Council (CNPq). This manuscript is based on the master’s thesis presented by the first author to the Graduate Program in Psychology at Universidade Federal de São Carlos. This research was part of the scientific program of Instituto Nacional de Ciência e Tecnologia sobre Comportamento, Cognição e Ensino (INCT-ECCE), supported by CNPq (Grant 573972/2008-7) and FAPESP (Grant 2008/57705-8).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mizael, T.M., de Almeida, J.H., Silveira, C.C. et al. Changing Racial Bias by Transfer of Functions in Equivalence Classes. Psychol Rec 66, 451–462 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0185-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0185-0

Keywords

Navigation