Skip to main content
Log in

Book Review: Diagnostic Dilemmas in Developmental Disabilities: Fuzzy Margins at the Edges of Normality. An Essay Prompted by Thomas Sowell's New Book: The Einstein Syndrome

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Sowell's “Einstein children” are rare, extraordinarily gifted children, often the offspring of engineers or other highly educated musical families, in whom exceptional visual/spatial reasoning skills mature much earlier than language skills. Sowell states that speechless children with demonstrably superior nonverbal problem-solving skills and mathematical ability whose language comprehension is adequate have a favorable outlook with or without intervention, even if their social skills and behavior are suboptimal. He stresses that listening to parents' reports of their children's accomplishments at home provides more reliable information than questionnaires or formal one-time evaluations to distinguish between inability and unwillingness to speak or perform. He strongly objects to invasive and stressful tests for such children and questions the effectiveness of the many early interventions offered to children with developmental disorders. Certainly, providing a definite prognosis in very young children is hazardous because it is so subject to error, unless there is independent evidence for a serious neurologic problem with brain dysfunction or incontrovertible signs of truly exceptional cognitive ability. Early, individualized intervention is mandatory for children, no matter how bright, whose language comprehension is inadequate, and for those with troublesome behavioral traits bordering on or indicative of an autistic spectrum (pervasive developmental disorder/PDD) diagnosis. Children with isolated abilities who are functionally inept in every day life are not “Einstein children” and their deficits must be addressed promptly and specifically.

Sowell's book raises important issues about behaviorally based diagnoses. Developmental disorders are not diseases that one does or does not have but are behaviorally defined dimensional traits along a continuum with fuzzy edges and a wide range of severity. Therefore, there is no crisp partition between normalcy and disorder, and between disorders with different names yet shared features, even when there is no controversy regarding the identification of prototypic exemplars. The utility for practical and research reasons of agreed upon standard diagnostic criteria is not in question, provided the statistical rather than biological nature of the criteria is kept in mind. Most subtypes of disorders like autism, dyslexia, or attention deficit are defined by arbitrary cuts in a bell-shaped spectrum of disability, which in no way invalidates their organic, neurologic basis. Developmental disorders are rarely the consequences of single gene defects or of definable postnatal insults; they have many causes that vary among individuals. Like many complex human traits, they are the expression of extremely complex multigenic influences on brain development. Because single gene defects are rare causes of these disorders, medical testing needs to be constrained by the history and examination. Intervention must address each child's particular needs and is, for the most part, educational.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

REFERENCES

  • Allen, D. A., & Rapin, I. (1992). Autistic children are also dysphasic. In H. Naruse and E. Ornitz (Eds.), Neurobiology of Infantile Autism (pp. 73–80), Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Excerpta Medica.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fourth edition, Text revision DSM IV-TR. (4th ed.), (pp. 69–84), Washington DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study. Psychological Medicine, 1, 63–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997). Is autism an extreme form of the male brain? Advances in Infancy Research, 11, 193–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1999). Plasticity, localization and language development. In S. Broman and J. M. Fletcher (Eds.), The changing nervous system: Neurobehavioral consequences of early brain development (pp. 214–253). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Mills, D., Galaburda, A., & Korenberg, J. R. (1999). Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Trends in Neurosciences, 22, 197–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1989). Autism, Asperger's syndrome and semanticpragmatic disorder: Where are the boundaries? British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24, 107–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J. (1998). Clinical Forum: SPD as a distinct diagnostic entity: Logical considerations and directions for future research. International Journal Language & Communication Disorders, 33, 71–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, M., & Rapin, I. (1997). Communication in autistic children. In P. J. Accardo, B. K. Shapiro, and A. J. Capute (Eds.), Behavior belongs in the brain: Neurobehavioral syndromes (pp. 97–111). Timonium, MD: York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek, G. T., Cook, E. H. Jr., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., Gravel, J. S., Johnson, C. P., Kallen, R. J., Levy, S. E., Minshew, N. J., Ozonoff, S., Prizant, B., Rapin, I., Rogers, S. J., Stone, W. L., Teplin, S., Tuchman, R. F., & Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Practice parameter: Screening and diagnosis of autism. Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurology Society. Neurology, 55, 468–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C. (1998). Chromosomal disorders and autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 415–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures and other reports from my life with autism. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hou, C., Miller, B. L., Cummings, J. L., Goldberg, M., Mychack, P., Bottino, V., & Benson, D. F. (2000). Artistic savants. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 13, 29–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, H. C. (1992). Measurement of reliability for categorical data in medical research. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 1, 183–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Couteur, A., Bailey, A., Best, S., Pickles, A., Robertson, S., Gottesman, I., & Rutter, M. (1996). A broader phenotype of autism: The clinical spectrum in twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 785–801.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. K. (1996). Musical savants: Exceptional skill in the mentally retarded. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nass, R. (1997). Language development in children with congenital strokes. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 4, 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H. J., & Bavelier, D. (1998). Neural organization and plasticity of language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 254–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M., Eisenmajer, R., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., Ong, B., & Dowe, D. (1998). Are there subgroups within the autistic spectrum? A cluster analysis of a group of children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39, 893–902.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. E. (1994). Visualizing the mind: Strategies of cognitive science and techniques of modern brain imaging open a window to the neural systems responsible for thought. Scientific American, 270, 58–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. E. (2000). Functional imaging in cognitive neuroscience. In M. J. Farah and T. E. Feinberg (Eds.), Patient-based approaches to cognitive neuroscience (pp. 35–52), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritvo, E. R., Jorde, L. B., Mason-Brothers, A., Freeman, B. J., Pingree, C., Jones, M. B., McMahon, W. M., Petersen, P. B., Jenson, W. R., & Mo, A. (1989). The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: Recurrence risk estimates and genetic counseling. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 1032–1036.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L., & Sceery, W. R. (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychiatric, social, and behavioral outcomes. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 465–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smalley, S. L. (1998). Behavioral genetics '97—Genetic influences in childhood-onset psychiatric disorders: Autism and attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Human Genetics, 60, 1276–1282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sowell, T. (1997). Late-talking children. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language-impaired preschoolers: a follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 407–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F., Cook, E., Jr., Pomeroy, J., Realmuto, G., & Tanguay, P. (1999). Summary of the Practice Parameters for the Assessment and Treatment of Children, Adolescents, and Adults with Autism and other Pervasive Developmental Disorders. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1611–1616.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P., Lord, C., Campbell, M., Freeman, B. J., Cichetti, D. V., Rutter, M., Kline, W., et al. (1994). Field trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1361–1367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Practitioner review: Early developmental language delay—what, if anything, should the clinician do about it? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 4, 613–648.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (1991). The relationship between Asperger's syndrome and Kanner's autism. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and asperger's syndrome (pp. 93–121). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witelson, S. F., Kigar, D. L., & Harvey, T. (1999). The exceptional brain of Albert Einstein. Lancet, 353, 2149–2153.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. (10th ed.). Geneva, CH: World Health Organization.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rapin, I. Book Review: Diagnostic Dilemmas in Developmental Disabilities: Fuzzy Margins at the Edges of Normality. An Essay Prompted by Thomas Sowell's New Book: The Einstein Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 32, 49–57 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1017956224167

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1017956224167

Navigation