Skip to main content
Log in

Do ‘Mind over Muscle’ Strategies Work?

Examining the Effects of Attentional Association and Dissociation on Exertional, Affective and Physiological Responses to Exercise

  • Review Article
  • Associative and Dissociative Strategies
  • Published:
Sports Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite the well established physical and psychological benefits derived from leading a physically active life, rates of sedentary behaviour remain high. Dropout and non-compliance are major contributors to the problem of physical inactivity. Perceptions of exertion, affective responses (e.g. displeasure or discomfort), and physiological stress could make the exercise experience aversive, particularly for beginners. Shifting one’s attentional focus towards environmental stimuli (dissociation) instead of one’s body (association) has been theorized to enhance psychological responses and attenuate physiological stress. Research evidence on the effectiveness of attentional focus strategies, however, has been perplexing, covering the entire gamut of possible outcomes (association and dissociation having been shown to be both effective and ineffective). This article examines the effects of manipulations of attentional focus on exertional and affective responses, as well as on exercise economy and tolerance. The possible roles of the characteristics of the exercise stimulus (intensity, duration) and the exercise participants, methodological issues, and limitations of experimental designs are discussed. In particular, the critical role of exercise intensity is emphasized. Dissociative strategies may be more effective in reducing perceptions of exertion and enhancing affective responses at low to moderate exercise intensities, but their effectiveness may be diminished at higher and nearmaximal levels, at which physiological cues dominate. Conversely, associative strategies could enable the exerciser to regulate intensity to avoid injury or overexertion. Thus, depending on intensity, both strategies have a place in the ‘toolbox’ of the public health or exercise practitioner as methods of enhancing the exercise experience and promoting long-term compliance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. United States Department of Health and Human Services. Physical activity and health: a report from the Surgeon General. Atlanta (GA): Department of Health andHuman Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Office of Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, 1996

  2. Armstrong T, Bauman A, Davies J. Physical activity patterns of Australian adults: results of the 1999 National Physical Activity Survey. Canberra (ACT): Australia nInstitute of Health and Welfare, 2000

    Google Scholar 

  3. Canadian Fitness and Lifestyle Research Institute. 2003 physical activity monitor. Ontario: Health Canada, 2003

  4. Sproston K, Primatesta P. Health survey for England. London: The Stationary Office, 2004

    Google Scholar 

  5. Mokdad AH, Marks JS, Stroup DF, et al. Actual causes of death in the United States, 2000. JAMA 2005; 293: 293–4

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Dishman RK, Buckworth J. Increasing physical activity: a quantitative synthesis. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1996; 28: 706–19

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Ekkekakis P. The study of affective responses to acute exercise: the dual-mode model. In: Stelter R, Roessler KK. editors. New approaches to exercise and sport psychology. Oxford: Meyer & Meyer Sport, 2005: 119–49

    Google Scholar 

  8. Ekkekakis P, Lind E. Exercise does not feel the same when you are overweight: the impact of self-selected and imposed exercise intensity on affect and exertion. Int JObesity (Lond) 2006; 30: 652–60

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Acevedo EO, Kraemer RR, Haltom RW, et al. Perceptual responses proximal to the onset of blood lactate accumulation. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 2003; 6: 267–73

    Google Scholar 

  10. Bixby WR, Spalding TW, Hatfield BD. Temporal dynamics and dimensional specificity of the affective responseto exercise of varying intensity: differing pathways to acommon outcome. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2001; 23: 171–90

    Google Scholar 

  11. Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, Petruzzello SJ. Practical markers of the transition from aerobic to anaerobic metabolismduring exercise: rationale and a case for affect-baseexercise prescription. Prev Med 2004; 38: 149–59

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hall EE, Ekkekakis P, Petruzzello SJ. The affective beneficence of vigorous exercise revisited. Br J Health Psychol 2002; 7: 47–66

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Cox KL, Burke V, Gorely TJ, et al. Controlled comparison of retention and adherence in home- vs center-initiated exercise interventions in women ages 40–65 years: the S.W.E.A.T. study (Sedentary Women Exercise Adherence Trial). Prev Med 2003; 36: 17–29

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Epstein LH, Koeske R, Wing RR. Adherence to exercise in obese children. J Card Rehabil 1984; 4: 185–95

    Google Scholar 

  15. Lee JY, Jensen BE, Oberman A, et al. Adherence in the training levels comparison trial. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1996; 28: 47–52

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Perri MG, Anton SD, Durning PE, et al. Adherence to exercise prescriptions: effects of prescribing moderateversus higher levels of intensity and frequency. Health Psychol 2002; 21: 452–8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sallis JF, Haskell WL, Fortmann SP, et al. Predictors of adoption and maintenance of physical activity in a community sample. Prev Med 1986; 15: 331–41

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Williams DM, Dunsiger S, Ciccolo JT, et al. Acute affective response to a moderate-intensity exercise stimulus predictsphysical activity participation 6 and 12 months later. Psych Sport Exerc 2008; 9: 231–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Emmons RA, Diener E. A goal-affect analysis of everyday situational choices. J Res Pers 1986; 20: 309–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Rejeski WJ, Kenney EA. Fitness motivation: preventing participant dropout. Champaign (IL): Human Kinetics, 1998

    Google Scholar 

  21. Leith LM. Exercising your way to better mental health. Morgantown (WV): Fitness Information Technology, Inc., 1998

    Google Scholar 

  22. Weinberg RS, Smith J, Jackson A, et al. Effect of association, dissociation and positive self-talk strategies on endurance performance. Can J Appl Sport Sci 1984; 9: 25–32

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Morgan WP. Mind of the marathoner. Psychol Today 1978; 11: 38–49

    Google Scholar 

  24. Schomer HH. Mental strategies and the perception of effort of marathon runners. Int J Sport Psychol 1986; 17: 41–59

    Google Scholar 

  25. Nideffer RM. The ethics and practice of applied sport psychology. New York (NY): Mouvement Publications, 1981

    Google Scholar 

  26. Stevinson CD, Biddle SJH. Cognitive orientations in marathon running and “hitting the wall ” Br J Sports Med 1998; 32: 229–35

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Stevinson CD, Biddle SJH. Cognitive strategies in running: a response to Masters and Ogles (1998). Sport Psych 1999; 13: 235–6

    Google Scholar 

  28. Summers JJ, Machin VJ, Sargent GI. Psychosocial factors related to marathon running. J Sport Psychol 1983; 5: 314–31

    Google Scholar 

  29. Summers JJ, Sargent GI, Levey AJ, et al. Middle-aged, non-elite marathon runners: a profile. Percept Mot Skills 1982; 54: 963–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Buman MP, Brewer BW, Cornelius AE, et al. Hitting the wall in the marathon: phenomenological characteristicsand associations with expectancy, gender, and running history. Psych Sport Exerc 2008; 9: 177–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Pennebaker JW, Lightner JM. Competition of internal and external information in an exercise setting. J Pers Soc Psychol 1980; 39: 165–74

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Fillingim RB, Fine MA. The effects of internal versus external information processing on symptom perception inan exercise setting. Health Psychol 1986; 5: 115–23

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Fillingim RB, Roth DL, Haley WE. The effects of distraction on the perception of exercise-induced symptoms. J Psychosom Res 1989; 33: 241–8

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Padgett VR, Hill AK. Maximizing athletic performance in endurance events: a comparison of cognitive strategies. J Appl Soc Psychol 1989; 19: 331–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Acevedo EO, Dzewaltowski DA, Gill DL, et al. Cognitive orientations of ultramarathoners. Sport Psychol 1992; 6: 242–52

    Google Scholar 

  36. Freischlag J. Selected psycho-social characteristics of marathoners. Int J Sport Psychol 1981; 12: 282–8

    Google Scholar 

  37. Hollander DB, Acevedo EO. Successful English Channel swimming: the peak experience. Sport Psych 2000; 14: 1–16

    Google Scholar 

  38. Baker J, Côté J, Deakin J. Cognitive characteristics of expert, middle of the pack, and back of the packultra-endurance triathletes. Psych Sport Exer 2005; 6: 551–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Goode KT, Roth DL. Factor analysis of cognitions during running: association with mood change. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1993; 15: 375–89

    Google Scholar 

  40. Bachman AD, Brewer BW, Petitpas AJ. Situation specificity of cognitions during running: replication and extension. J Appl Sport Psychol 1997; 9: 204–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Butryn TM, Furst DM. The effects of park and urban setting on the moods and cognitive strategies of female runners. J Sport Beh 2003; 26 (4): 335–55

    Google Scholar 

  42. LaCaille RA, Masters KS, Heath EM. Effects of cognitive strategy and exercise setting on running performance,perceived exertion, affect, and satisfaction. Psych Sport Exer 2004; 5: 461–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Brewer BW, Van Raalte JL, Linder DE. Attentional focus and endurance performance. Appl Res Coaching Athl Ann 1996; 11: 1–14

    Google Scholar 

  44. Masters KS, Ogles BM. The relations of cognitive strategies with injury, motivation, and performance amongmarathon runners: results from two studies. J Appl Sport Psychol 1998; 10: 281–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Connolly CT, Janelle CM. Attentional strategies in rowing: performance, perceived exertion, and gender considerations. J Appl Sport Psychol 2003; 15: 195–212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Masters KS, Ogles BM, Jolton JA. The development of an instrument to measure motivation for marathon running:the Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS). Res Q Exerc Sport 1993; 64: 134–43

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Tenenbaum G, Fogarty G, Stewart E, et al. Perceived discomfort in running: scale development and theoretical considerations. J Sports Sci 1999; 17: 183–96

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Carmack MA, Martens R. Measuring commitment to running: a survey of runners’ attitudes and mental states. J Sport Psychol 1979; 1: 25–42

    Google Scholar 

  49. Morgan WP, Pollock ML. Psychologic characterization of the elite distance runner. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1977; 301: 382–403

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Morgan WP, Horstman DH, Cymerman A, et al. Facilitation of physical performance by means of cognitive strategy. Cognit Ther Res 1983; 7: 251–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Morgan WP, O’Connor PJ, Ellickson KA, et al. Personality structure, mood states, and performance in elite maledistance runners. Int J Sport Psychol 1988; 19: 247–63

    Google Scholar 

  52. Crews DJ. Psychological state and running economy. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1992; 24: 475–82

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Hardy L, Nelson D. Self-regulation training in sport and work. Ergonomics 1988; 31: 1573–83

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Laasch C. Cognitive strategies and long-distance running. Imag Cognit Pers 1994-1995; 14: 317–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Masters KS, Ogles BM. Associative and dissociative cognitive strategies in exercise and running: 20 years later, what do we know? Sport Psych 1998; 12: 253–70

    Google Scholar 

  56. Moran AP. Cognitive style constructs in sport: explanatory and attentional processes in athletes. Int J Educ Res 1998; 29: 277–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Rose JMC. Association and dissociation cognitive strategies in endurance performance: a brief review. In: Watkins J, Reilly T, Burwitz L, editors. Sports science: proceedings of the VIII Commonwealth and International Conference on Sport, Physical Education, Dance, Recreation and Health. New York (NY): E. & F.N. Spon, 1986: 229–36

    Google Scholar 

  58. Leventhal H. A perceptual-motor processing model of emotion. In: Pliner P, Blankstein RK, Spigel IM, editors. Perception of emotion in self and others: advances in the study of communication and affect. New York (NY): Plenum, 1979: 1–46

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  59. Leventhal H, Everhart D. Emotion pain, and physical illness. In: Izard CE, editor. Emotion and psychopathology. New York (NY): Plenum, 1979: 263–99

    Google Scholar 

  60. Rejeski WJ. The perception of exertion: a social psychophysiological integration. J Sport Psychol 1981; 4: 305–20

    Google Scholar 

  61. Rejeski WJ. Perceived exertion: an active or passive process? J Sport Psychol 1985; 7: 371–8

    Google Scholar 

  62. Tenenbaum G. A social-cognitive perspective of perceived exertion and exertion tolerance. In: Singer RN, Hausenblas HA, Janelle C, editors. Handbook of sport psychology. New York (NY): Wiley, 2001: 810–22

    Google Scholar 

  63. Ekkekakis P. Pleasure and displeasure from the body: perspectives from exercise. Cogn Emot 2003; 17: 213–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Hall EE, Ekkekakis P, Petruzzello SJ. Is the relationship of RPE to psychological factors intensity-dependent? Med Sci Sports Exerc 2005; 37: 1365–73

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Ekkekakis P, Acevedo EO. Affective responses to acute exercise: toward a psychobiological dose-response model. In: Acevedo EO, Ekkekakis P, editors. Psycho biology of physical activity. Champaign (IL): Human Kinetics, 2006: 91–109

    Google Scholar 

  66. Durtschi SK, Weiss MR. Psychological characteristics of elite and nonelite marathon runners. In: Landers DM, editor. The 1984 Olympic Scientific Congress proceedings: sport and elite performers. Champaign (IL): Human Kinetics, 1984: 73–80

    Google Scholar 

  67. McDonald D, Kirkby RJ. Use of dissociation strategies when running becomes difficult: levels of ability andgender differences. Eur J High Ability 1995; 6: 73–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Silva III JM, Appelbaum MI. Association-dissociation patterns of United States Olympic marathon trial contestants. Cognit Ther Res 1989; 13: 185–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Antonini-Philippe R, Reynes E, Bruant G. Cognitive strategy and ability in endurance activities. Percept Mot Skills 2003; 96: 510–16

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Tenenbaum G, Connolly CT. Attention allocation under varied workload and effort perception in rowers. Psych Sport Exerc 2008; 9: 704–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Wrisberg CA, Pein RL. Past running experience as a mediator of the attentional focus of male and female recreational runners. Percept Mot Skills 1990; 70: 427–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Ogles BM, Lynn SJ, Masters KS, et al. Runners’ cognitive strategies and motivations: absorption, fantasy style, anddissociative experiences. Imag Cognit Pers 1993/1994; 13: 163–74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Callen KE. Mental and emotional aspects of long-distance running. Psychosomatics 1983; 24: 133–41

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Okwumabua TM. Psychological and physical contributions to marathon performance: an exploratory investigation. J Sport Beh 1985; 8: 163–71

    Google Scholar 

  75. Okwumabua TM, Meyers AW, Santille L. A demographic and cognitive profile of master runners. J Sport Beh 1987; 11: 212–20

    Google Scholar 

  76. Ungerleider S, Golding JM, Porter K, et al. An exploratory examination of cognitive strategies used by master’s trackand field athletes. Sport Psychol 1989; 3: 245–53

    Google Scholar 

  77. Ebbeck V. Sources of performance information in the exercise setting. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1990; 12: 56–65

    Google Scholar 

  78. Schomer HH, Connolly MJ. Cognitive strategies used by marathoners in each quartile of a training run. S Afr J Res Sport Phys Educ Rec 2002; 24: 87–99

    Google Scholar 

  79. Kirkby RJ. Ultraendurance running: a case study. Int J Sport Psychol 1996; 27: 109–16

    Google Scholar 

  80. Sachs ML. The mind of the runner: cognitive strategies used during running. In: Sachs ML, Buffone GW, editors. Running as therapy: an integrated approach. Lincoln(NE): University of Nebraska Press, 1984: 288–303

    Google Scholar 

  81. Sacks MH, Milvy P, Perry SW, et al. Mental status and psychological coping during a 100-mile race. In: Sacks MH, Sachs ML, editors. Psychology of running. Champaign (IL): Human Kinetics, 1981: 166–75

    Google Scholar 

  82. Moran AP. The psychology of concentration in sport performers: a cognitive analysis. East Sussex (UK): Psychology Press, 1996

    Google Scholar 

  83. Schomer HH. The relationship between cognitive strategies and perceived effort of marathon runners. S Afr J Res Sport Phys Educ Rec 1987; 10: 37–64

    Google Scholar 

  84. Schomer HH. Mental strategy training programme for marathon runners. Int J Sport Psychol 1987; 18: 133–51

    Google Scholar 

  85. Schomer HH. A cognitive strategy training programme for marathon runners: ten case studies. S Afr J Res Sport Phys Educ Rec 1990; 134: 47–78

    Google Scholar 

  86. Masters KS, Lambert MJ. The relations between cognitive coping strategies, reasons for running, injury, and performanceof marathon runners. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1989; 11: 161–70

    Google Scholar 

  87. Wrisberg CA, Franks BD, Birdwell MW, et al. Physiological and psychological responses to exercise with aninduced attentional focus. Percept Mot Skills 1988; 66: 603–16

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  88. Hassmèn P, Koivula N. Ratings of perceived exertion by women with internal or external locus of control. J Gen Psychol 1996; 123: 297–307

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Koivula N, Hassmen P. Central, local, and overall ratings of perceived exertion during cycling and running bywomen with an external or internal locus of control. J Gen Psychol 1998; 125: 17–29

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  90. Baden DA, McLean TL, Tucker R, et al. Effect of anticipation during unknown or unexpected exercise durationon rating of perceived exertion, affect, and physiological function. Br J Sports Med 2005; 39: 742–6

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  91. Tammen VV. Elite middle and long distance runner’s associative/ dissociative coping. J Appl Sport Psychol 1996; 8: 1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Beaudoin CM, Crews DJ, Morgan DW. Influence of psychogenic factors during a prolonged maximal run. J Sport Beh 1998; 21: 377–81

    Google Scholar 

  93. Russell WD, Weeks DL. Attentional style in ratings of perceived exertion during physical exercise. Percept Mot Skills 1994; 78: 779–83

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  94. Deligniéres D, Brisswalter J. Influence of an added perceptual motor task on perceived exertion: a testof the dissociation effect. Percept Mot Skills 1994; 78: 855–58

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Baden DA, Warwick-Evans L, Lakomy J. Am I nearly there? The effect of anticipated running distance onperceived exertion and attentional focus. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2004; 26: 215–31

    Google Scholar 

  96. Stanley CT, Pargman D, Tenenbaum G. The effect of attentional coping strategies on perceived exertion in acycling task. J Appl Sport Psychol 2007; 19: 352–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Johnson JH, Siegel DS. Active vs passive attentional manipulation and multidimensional perceptions of exerciseintensity. Can J Sport Sci 1987; 12: 41–5

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  98. Couture RT, Jerome W, Tihanyi J. Can associative and dissociative strategies affect the swimming performance ofrecreational swimmers? Sport Psych 1999; 13: 334–43

    Google Scholar 

  99. Couture RT, Tihanyi J, St Aubin M. Can performance in a distance swim be improved by increasing a preferred cognitive thinking strategy? Sport J 2003; 6 (2): 1–6

    Google Scholar 

  100. Harte JL, Eifert GH. The effects of running, environment, and attentional focus on athletes’ catecholamine andcortisol levels and mood. Psycho physiology 1995; 32: 49–54

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  101. Siegal D, Johnson J, Davis C. Attention and perception of intensity of work. Percept Mot Skills 1981; 53: 331–7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Stamford BA, Weltman A, Foulke E. Information processing and perceived effort during bicycle ergometer work [abstract]. Med Sci Sport 1979; 11: 78

    Google Scholar 

  103. Couture RT, Singh M, Lee W, et al. The effect of mental training on the performance of military endurance tasksin the Canadian infantry. Int J Sports Psychol 1994; 25: 144–57

    Google Scholar 

  104. Franks BD, Myers BC. Effects of talking on exercise tolerance. Res Q Exerc Sport 1984; 55: 237–41

    Google Scholar 

  105. Welch AS, Hulley A, Ferguson C, et al. Affective responses of inactive women to a maximal incremental exercise test:a test of the dual-mode model. Psych Sport Exer 2007; 8: 401–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Hardy CJ, Rejeski WL. Not what, but how one feels: the measurement of affect during exercise. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1989; 11: 304–17

    Google Scholar 

  107. Cioffi D. Sensory awareness versus sensory impression: affect and attention interact to produce somatic meaning. Cogn Emot 1991; 5: 275–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Gauvin L, Rejeski WL. The exercise-induced feeling inventory: development and initial validation. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1993; 15: 403–23

    Google Scholar 

  109. Blanchard CM, Rodgers WM, Gauvin L. The influence of exercise duration and cognitions during running on feelingstates in an indoor running track environment. Psych Sport Exer 2004; 5: 119–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Masters KS. Hypnotic susceptibility, cognitive dissociation, and runners high in a sample of marathon runners. Am J Clin Hypn 1992; 34: 193–201

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  111. Pennebaker JW, Skelton JA. Psychological parameters of physical symptoms. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1978; 4: 524–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  112. Côté J, Salmela J, Papathanasopoulu KP. Effects of progressive exercise on attentional focus. Percept Mot Skills 1992; 75: 351–4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Salmela JH, Ndoye OD. Cognitive distortions during progressive exercise. Percept Mot Skills 1986; 63: 1067–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Stetson BA, Frommelt SJ, Boutelle KN, et al. Exerciserelated thoughts in cardiac exercise programs: a study ofexercise-adherent cardiac rehabilitation patients. Int J Rehabil Health 1995; 1: 125–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Martin JE, Dubbert PM, Katell AD, et al. Behavioral control of exercise in sedentary adults: studies 1 through 6. J Con Clin Psychol 1984; 52: 795–811

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  116. Welsh MC, Labbé EE, Delaney D. Cognitive strategies and personality variables in adherence to exercise. Psychol Rep 1991; 68: 1327–35

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  117. Saintsing DE, Richman CL, Bergey DB. Effects of three cognitive strategies on long-distance running. Bull Psychon Soc 1988; 26: 34–6

    Google Scholar 

  118. Okwumabua TM, Meyers AW, Schleser R, et al. Cognitive strategies and running performance: an exploratory study. Cognit Ther Res 1983; 7: 363–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  119. Spink KS, Longhurst K. Cognitive strategies and swimming performances: an exploratory study. Aust J Sci Med Sport 1986; 18: 9–13

    Google Scholar 

  120. Rushall BS, Shewchuk ML. Effects of thought content instructions on swimming performance. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 1989; 29: 326–34

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  121. Baghurst T, Thierry G, Holder T. Evidence for a relationship between attentional styles and effective cognitivestrategies during performance. Athl Insight 2004; 6 (1): 36–51

    Google Scholar 

  122. Pargman D. Individual differences: cognitive and perceptual styles. In: Singer RN, Murphey M, Tennant LK, editors. Handbook of sport psychology. New York (NY): Macmillan, 1993: 379–401

    Google Scholar 

  123. Hutchinson JC, Tenenbaum G. Attention focus during physical effort: the mediating role of task intensity. Psych Sport Exerc 2007; 8: 233–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  124. Gontang A, Clitsome T, Kostrubala T. A psychological study of 50 sub-3-hour marathoners. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1977; 301: 1020–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  125. Hartung GH, Farge EJ. Personality and physiological traits in middle-aged runners and joggers. J Gerontol 1977; 32: 541–8

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  126. Hatfield BD, Spalding TW, Mahon AD, et al. The effect of psychological strategies upon cardiorespiratory andmuscular activity during treadmill running. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1992; 24: 218–25

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  127. Rushall BS, Hall M, Roux L, et al. Effects of three types of thought content instructions on skiing performance. Sport Psych 1988; 2: 283–97

    Google Scholar 

  128. Smith AL, Gill DL, Crews DJ, et al. Attentional strategy use by experienced long distance runners: physiological andpsychological effects. Res Q Exerc Sport 1995; 66: 142–50

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  129. Martin JJ, Craib M, Mitchell V. The relationships of anxiety and self-attention to running economy in competitivemale distance runners. J Sports Sci 1995; 13: 371–6

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  130. Hatfield BD, Spalding TW, Mahon AD, et al. Ventilatory economy as a function of attentional self-focus during treadmill running [abstract]. Psycho physiology 1986; 23: 440–1

    Google Scholar 

  131. Simes WE. A new look at association/dissociation in long distance runners. Running Psychol 1982; 4: 1–3

    Google Scholar 

  132. Donohue B, Barnhart R, Covassin T, et al. The development and initial evaluation of two promising mental preparatorymethods in a sample of female cross countryrunners. J Sport Beh 2001; 24: 19–30

    Google Scholar 

  133. Miller A, Donohue B. The development and controlled evaluation of athletic mental preparation strategies inhigh school distance runners. J Appl Sport Psychol 2003; 15: 321–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  134. Scott LM, Scott D, Bedic SP, et al. The effect of associative and dissociative strategies on rowing ergometer performance. Sport Psych 1999; 13: 57–68

    Google Scholar 

  135. Clingman JM, Hilliard DV. Race walkers quicken their pace by tuning in, not stepping out. Sport Psych 1990; 4: 25–32

    Google Scholar 

  136. Goudas M, Theodorakis Y, Laparidis K. The effect of external versus internal types of feedback and goalsetting on endurance performance. Athl Insight 2007; 9 (3): 57–66

    Google Scholar 

  137. Nietfield JL. An examination of metacognitive strategy use and monitoring skills by competitive middle-distancerunners. J Appl Sport Psychol 2003; 15: 307–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  138. Guyot WG. Psychological and medical factors associated with pain running. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 1991; 31: 452–60

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  139. Takai K. Cognitive strategies and recall of pace by longdistance runners. Percept Mot Skills 1998; 86: 763–70

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  140. Siegal D, Johnson J, Kline G. Attentional load and the reproduction of physical work. Res Q Exerc Sport 1984; 55: 146–52

    Google Scholar 

  141. Cioffi D, Holloway J. Delayed costs of suppressed pain. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993; 64: 274–82

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  142. Orlick T. Psyching for marathons: in pursuit of excellence. Ottawa (ON): Canadian Coaching Association, 1980

    Google Scholar 

  143. Ungerleider S. Mental training for peak performance. Emmaus (PA): Rodale Press, Inc., 1996

    Google Scholar 

  144. Berger B. Running strategies for women and men. In: Sachs ML, Buffone GW, editors. Running as therapy: an integrated approach. Lincoln (NE): University of Nebraska Press, 1984: 23–62

    Google Scholar 

  145. Roth WT. Some motivational aspects of exercise. J Sports Med 1974; 14: 40–7

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  146. Rejeski WJ, Kenney EA. Distracting attention from fatigue: does task complexity make a difference? J Sport Exerc Psychol 1987; 9 (1): 66–73

    Google Scholar 

  147. Johnson JH, Siegel DS. Effects of association and dissociation on effort perception. J Sport Beh 1992; 15: 119–29

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Dr Spiridoula Vazou for critical insights and helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier version of the manuscript. No sources of funding were used to assist in the preparation of this review. The authors have no conflicts of interest that are directly relevant to the content of this review.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Panteleimon Ekkekakis.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lind, E., Welch, A.S. & Ekkekakis, P. Do ‘Mind over Muscle’ Strategies Work?. Sports Med 39, 743–764 (2009). https://doi.org/10.2165/11315120-000000000-00000

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.2165/11315120-000000000-00000

Keywords

Navigation