Introduction

In psychology and biology, cognition refers to the mechanisms by which animals acquire, process, store, and act on information from the environment (Shettleworth, 2009). In their environment, animals constantly face various situations to be solved. Cognitive abilities thus play a crucial role in their daily life as they enable certain common key functions such as foraging, predator avoidance, mate-choice, navigation, and orientation (Gierszewski, Bleckmann, & Schluessel, 2013). To date, most of what we know about animals’ cognitive skills focuses on higher vertebrates (primates: Sidman et al., 1982; Vonk, 2003; dolphins: Herman, Gory, Hovancik, & Bradshaw, 1989), but very few comprehensive studies focused on farmed animals and even fewer on farmed-fish species. However, the attribution of cognitive abilities to animals precisely relates to the way in which the animal will be considered and used. Among farmed animals, rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) is the first continental fish species to be commercially produced in Europe (FEAP, 2017). Increasing public concern for the welfare of fish species used for food consumption has highlighted the need to better understand the cognitive status of fish. Cognitive processes are key components of fish welfare under farming conditions: individuals need to anticipate specific events (food distribution or netting) or to habituate to stressful situations in order to reduce stress triggered by an unpredictable environment (Cañon Jones, Noble, Damsgård & Pearce, 2012; Colson et al., 2019; Martins et al., 2012). A better knowledge of the rainbow trout’s cognitive abilities would help to find ways to improve their welfare, by developing appropriate – species-specific – ethological-based tools, such as cognitive enrichments for future farming systems. Cognitive enrichment, also known as occupational or psychological enrichment, represents the possibility for animals to meet moderate challenges by using their cognitive abilities and/or to actively interact with their environment (Manteuffel, Langbein & Puppe, 2009; Meehan & Mench, 2007; Oesterwind, Nürnberg, Puppe & Langbein, 2016). In this way, an animal is offered the possibility to anticipate and/or to control positive or negative events in order to stimulate its cognition by choosing appropriate behavior.

Fish cognition has begun to be investigated over the last few decades, and studies have revealed that many fish species are endowed with impressive learning skills, comparable to those of non-human primates (Bshary, Wickler, & Fricke, 2002). Certain fish species have episodic-like memory (Ingraham, Anderson, Hurd & Hamilton, 2016), make transitive inferences about social relationships (Grosenick, Clement & Fernald, 2007), attend to their own reputation (Bshary & Grutter, 2006), use tools (Paśko, 2010; Gerullis & Schuster, 2014; Millot et al., 2014), solve numerical rules (Agrillo & Bisazza, 2014; DeLong, Barbato, O’Leary & Wilcox, 2017; Gomez-Laplaza & Gerlai, 2013), learn concepts (Gierszewski et al., 2013; Goldman & Shapiro, 1979; Zerbolio & Royalty, 1983), orient themselves using a map-like representation of their environment (Broglio, Rodriguez, & Salas, 2003; Rodriguez, Duran, Vargas, Torres, & Salas, 1994), and recognize conspecifics individually using visual cues (Clark & Stephenson, 1999; Culumber, 2015; Griffiths & Magurran, 1999; Johnston & Dixson, 2017; Roux et al., 2016; Satoh, Tanaka & Kohda, 2016). However, none of these cognitive abilities – except visual individual recognition (Höjesjö, Axelsson, Dahy, Gustavsson, & Johnsson, 2015) – was demonstrated in rainbow trout. Visual cues are essential for fish when identifying familiar and unfamiliar individuals, engaging in social learning, avoiding predators, foraging, and navigating (Ioannou, Couzin, James, Croft, & Krause, 2011). Despite a poor visual acuity compared to humans (Ben-Simon, Ben-Shahar, Vasserman, Ben-Tov, & Segev, 2012; Champ, Wallis, Vorobyev, Siebeck, & Marshall, 2014; Hester, 1968; Neumeyer, 2003), some teleost fish are able to discriminate between colors (Oncorhynchus mykiss: Luchiari & Pirhonen, 2008; Danio rerio: Parker et al., 2012), two- (2-D) and three-dimensional (3-D) shapes and even illusory shape surfaces (Gnathonemus petersii: Schuster et al., 2002; Pseudotropheus sp.: Schluessel et al., 2012; Pomacentrus amboinensis: Siebeck et al., 2009; Carassius auratus: Wyzisk et al., 2007).

Most of the tests evaluating fish cognitive abilities are discrimination procedures where fish are previously conditioned to associate and recognize a particular stimulus with positive (S+: food reward) or negative (S-: electric shocks) stimuli (Goldman & Shapiro, 1979; Zerbolio & Royalty, 1983). The most commonly used test is the alternative forced-choice (AFC) test, which consists of simultaneously presenting different visual stimuli to the subject, one being rewarded. Fish are frequently tested using the 2-AFC test, a two-choice discrimination test (Danio rerio: Bloch et al., 2019; Parker et al., 2012; Pseudotropheus sp.: Schluessel, Fricke, & Bleckmann, 2012; Xenotoca eiseni: Sovrano & Bisazza, 2008). To access the discrimination performance, studies generally use discrete operant responses, such as approaching sensors (Siebeck, Litherland, & Wallis, 2009), time spent near positive stimuli (Ingraham et al., 2016; Kuroda, Mizutani, Cancado, & Podlesnik, 2017), and passing through a door to enter a rewarded chamber (Agrillo, Piffer, & Bisazza, 2010; Bloch, Froc, Pontiggia, & Yamamoto, 2019; Gomez-Laplaza, Díaz-Sotelo, & Gerlai, 2018; Mueller & Neuhauss, 2012; Parker et al., 2012; von der Emde & Fetz, 2007; Wyzisk & Neumeyer, 2007), but no studies used the ability of the fish to operate a lever as an active discrete operant response. This last method – also called instrumental conditioning procedure – allows recording the subject’s answer with accuracy, particularly when the setup is automated. Moreover, instrumental learning involves a complete panel of cognitive abilities, such as spatial orientation, object recognition, and temporal association of environmental cues (Jurado-Parras et al., 2013), and thus represents an opportunity for investigating cognitive abilities, still little studied in the target fish species chosen for the present experiment, i.e., the rainbow trout.

If we presume that rainbow trout’s vision is comparable to that of most of teleost fish species as discussed above, we do not know to which extent they are capable discriminating accurately between conspecifics and any other objects or living animals present in their environment or if they rather form categories on the basis of shared shapes, as shown in the African cichlid Pseudotropheus sp. (Schluessel et al., 2012). This capacity to generalize visual items using categories gives individuals a certain degree of flexibility to react appropriately in case of danger (categories “predator,” “shelter”), while hunting (category “prey”), or to ensure positive outcomes (category “reward”), and provide them with stability and safety in a context of changing environments (Franks, 2018).

The aim of this study was to explore the cognitive skills of a farmed fish species by (1) using an original operant conditioning automated device composed of self-feeders, in order to determine (2) whether rainbow trout, the first European continental fish production, are able to solve a simple 2-AFC task, and (3) whether they can recognize conspecifics specifically, or if they rather generalize learned photographs of conspecifics to any other fish species sharing a similar shape. For this purpose, we used a pool of four recurrent photographs of trout conspecifics as S+. After a first stage using four different blue shapes as S- which failed in eliciting correct conditioned responses, we then used distracting stimuli as S- according to three stages with increasing discrimination difficulty: stage 2: one black shape (star); stage 3: photograph of an object (among a pool of 60); stage 4: photograph of another fish species (among a pool of 60).

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

All experimental procedures used in this study were carried out in strict accordance with the European Directive 2010/63/EU on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes. They have been approved by the Rennes Ethics Committee for Animal Experimentation and have received authorization from the French Ministry of Education, Research and Innovation under agreement number 201806050954214-V1-APAFIS#15361.

Experimental animals

Female triploid rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) were obtained from fertilizations performed at INRA-PEIMA facilities (Sizun, France). Eggs were transferred between 1 and 20 h post-fertilization, and fish from these eggs were reared at the INRA-LPGP experimental facilities (Rennes, France). Between 50 and 180 days postfertilization, fish were reared in two tanks (72 L: 55 × 45 × 29 cm) containing 40 fish each. The density was always below 20 kg/m3. Tanks were supplied with plants, rocks, and PVC pipes since environmental enrichment is known to enhance cognitive capacities in fish (Salvanes et al., 2013; Strand et al., 2010). At 5 months, 15 fish (8 cm mean length) were randomly chosen and were identified with a PIT tag (1.4 × 8 mm; Tiny, Biolog-id, France) implanted above the dorsal fin under anesthesia (50 mg/L tricaine). The 15 fish were housed together in a 336-L tank (102.5 × 102.5 × 32 cm) at 12 ± 0.2 °C under a 12:12 h artificial photoperiod. Fish were fed daily at the end of the experiments with extruded and commercial flowing extruded pellets (39% protein and 24% lipid, 0.5-mm pellets, Le Gouessant, France). The feeding rate, composition, and diameter of the pellets was regularly adapted according to the fish growth rate. Experiments started after a 2-week acclimation period in the new rearing tank.

Apparatus

Subjects were tested individually in a 779-L test tank (104 × 100.5 × 74.5 cm) with the walls painted dark gray (RAL code: 7005). A transparent Plexiglas window (63 × 34 cm) included in the tank wall allowed the fish to see the screen placed behind it (Fig. 1). The screen used in this study was an LCD Dell REV A00 (17 in.: 60 × 34 cm) and displayed visual stimuli. To prevent any distractions, disruptions, or reflections from the screen, a dark sheet was laid behind the screen. Two (or three) self-feeders (removable if needed) were placed in the upper part, and the triggers were 4 cm from the window and submerged at approximately 9 cm depth in the water. Fish could reach the pressure needed by the trigger by doing a 5–6° tilt angle with any part of their body. Once the angle was reached, an electronic microchip located in the cylinder validated the answer. Self-feeders were connected to an automatic food distributor (Imetronic®, France) delivering coated appetitive pellets (Diana Aqua, France). Their diameter was changed according to fish weight and pellet granulometric recommendations. When a correct answer was recorded, two pellets were released into a funnel connected to a 1.5-cm plastic tube and then fell into the water in the middle of the tank at approximately 30 cm from the screen. Each reward corresponded to approximately 1/12th of the fish daily food intake. A camera placed above the experimental device (Samsung SCB-3000PH) and connected to a monitor (Ever-Focus Colour TFT LCD monitor) permitted the control of the entire real-time experiment at distance without disturbing trout behavior. A GoPro (HERO session) was introduced into the tank once during each stage to capture sequences for illustration purposes only (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Experimental tank used for the two-alternative forced-choice test task; b camera view from above; c GoPro view from inside the tank while stimuli from stage 3 were displayed (here, a calculator as S-)

Each experiment was managed by the “POLY M2S” software (Imetronic®), which allows users to choose the type of experiment, the number of trials, the intertrial interval (ITI), the different stimuli, and their position on the screen. The software also ensures that stimuli do not appear more than two consecutive times at the same position to prevent any side bias. All stimulus sequences were randomly presented (see General procedure).

General procedure

Fish were never tested more than once a day (one session per day). Experiments were undertaken between 9:00 and 15:00 and the fish were all fed at the same time at the end of the last session, resulting in an overnight food deprivation period varying from 18 h to 24 h, depending on fish running order.

Training to activate triggers

Fifteen trout were initially trained to become acclimated to the apparatus and activate the self-feeder triggers. For this habituation phase, three triggers (instead of two for the subsequent stages) were submerged 9 cm in order to increase their chance of being activated. At the fish entrance in the test area, a food reward was provided to make them associate netting as a positive event. During this habituation phase, the monitor displayed a gray screen (RGB code: #EEECEC) to reduce contrast with the tank color, and the fish received a food reward, regardless of the trigger activated as they were all the right choice. We tested the fish once a day (one session per day), 5 days a week. The first session consisted of five trials of 10 min each. If a trigger was activated before a 10-min delay, the fish received a reward, and the screen turned green (RGB code: #0F790F) for 60 s, which acts as a secondary positive reinforcement (Luchiari & Pirhonen, 2008; Zerbolio & Royalty, 1983). After that, a 10-s ITI with a gray screen occurred, and a new trial began. If the fish did not activate the trigger within the 10-min delay (cut-off), a food reward was still given, and a new trial started after the 10-s ITI.

In the following sessions, the procedure was the same as in session 1, but each session included ten trials of 10 min each, and no more reward was given after a cut-off. After each session, the trout was netted and joined its conspecifics. Fish were tested randomly. Individuals were considered to have successfully learned to activate the triggers once they activated one of them seven times (or more) over the ten trials in three consecutive sessions (≥70%; binomial: P < 0.001, N = 30 trials), ensuring the correct application of the operant conditioning device for all subsequent stages. Nine individuals (out of 15) correctly learned trigger activation (see Results section).

Pretraining to activate a trigger associated with a stimulus

The nine remaining subjects were trained to associate a trigger with the presence of one unique stimulus of different shapes and different colors (S+) displayed behind it on the screen for a total of 540 trials before starting the 2-AFC test. From this exercise until the end of the experiment, only two triggers were available, one being rewarded (right or left position). The S+ never appeared more than twice consecutively on the same side. In the case of correct answers, the fish received a reward, and the screen turned green for 60 s. If the incorrect trigger was activated, no food reward was given, and a red screen appeared (RGB code: #E90808) for 60 s, followed by a 10-s ITI before starting a new trial. This color acted as a positive punishment to enhance learning. After a cut-off, no food reward was given, and a new trial started after the ITI. Ten trials of 10 min each were conducted per session. An individual was considered to have successfully learned the task once S+ selection was significantly different from chance (50%) for two consecutive sessions (≥ 80%; binomial: P = 0.012, N = 20 trials).

Two-alternative forced-choice test (2-AFC) with photographs of conspecifics as S+

Only five of the nine individuals were used for the following tasks because one fish died and three individuals stopped being motivated, performing only cut-offs. The 2-AFC test consists of presenting two contrasting stimuli simultaneously, one being rewarded (right or left position) (Fig. 2). S+ represented a photograph of trout taken from a pool of four recurrent photographs of unknown conspecifics held in another tank (see Table 1 for the detail of the stimuli used). We chose photographs of conspecifics as biologically relevant stimuli since we know that rainbow trout are able to recognize conspecifics individually using visual cues (Höjesjö et al., 2015; Johnsson & Åkerman, 1998). The four S+ photographs were presented in equal numbers across the sessions, in a randomized order. A distracting stimulus (S-) was presented at 40 cm (horizontally) from the S+. The S- were: stage 1 – four different blue (RGB code: #1E90FF) shapes (star, triangle, square or circle); stage 2 – a unique black shape (star); stage 3 – a photograph of an object (among a pool of 60 objects); and stage 4 – a photograph of another fish species (from a pool of 60 photographs). Blue shapes were first chosen as S- in stage 1 since we presumed that shapes with an attractive color would counteract the possible natural preference for a biologically relevant stimulus as S+ (Oliveira, Silveira, Chacon & Luchiari, 2015; Roy Suriyampola, Flores, López, Hickey, Bhat, & Martins, 2019). Indeed, the color blue has been shown to be preferred (along with green) when given the choice to rainbow trout (Luchiari & Pirhonen, 2008; Maia, Ferguson, Volpato, & Braithwaite, 2017). In stage 2, we reduced to one the number of shapes used as S- because trout failed with four shapes as S- in stage 1, and other species succeeded when using only one or two shapes as S- for a similar study design (Gabor & Gerken, 2012; Langbein, Nurnberg, & Manteuffel, 2004; Schluessel et al., 2012). Additionally in stage 2, S- was chosen in a dark color (black) to increase the visual contrast between stimuli. The pool of objects used as S- in stage 3 allowed us to test whether trout only learned to avoid the black star in stage 2 or, conversely, to associate the photograph of a conspecific with a reward. Fish performed these stages in this order and moved on to the next stage as soon as they validated the learning criterion or if they reached the initially chosen maximum number of sessions. A maximum number of 30 sessions was established for stages 1–3, and ten sessions was specified for stage 4 (other fish species). Stage 4 assessed the ability of the fish to discriminate the recurrent photographs of conspecifics from other fish species or, on the contrary, to generalize the learned recurrent stimuli to other stimuli sharing similar features (fish shape). As the instruction given in stage 4 (discriminating between two images of fish) was considered to be ambiguous, we chose ten sessions as a maximum, because more than ten sessions to complete could have cancelled the initial positive association of an image of a trout with a reward and could have acted as a form of reversal learning (Kuroda et al., 2017; Lucon-Xiccato & Bisazza, 2014). The S+ and S- never appeared more than twice consecutively on the same side. In the case of correct answers, the fish received a reward, and the screen turned green for 60 s. If the incorrect trigger was activated, no food reward was given, and a red screen appeared for 60 s, followed by a 10-s ITI before starting a new trial. After a cut-off (no answer within 10 min), no food reward was given, and a new trial started after the ITI. For all stages, each session included 12 trials of 10 min each. An individual was considered to have successfully learned the task once an S+ selection was significantly different from chance (50%) for two consecutive sessions (≥ 75%; binomial: P = 0.011, N = 24 trials).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Successive exercises performed by fish in the two-alternative forced-choice test task after trigger habituation. S+ rewarded stimulus; S- examples of unrewarded stimuli. Stage 1: blue shapes as S-; Stage 2 (step 1): black star as S-; Stage 3: objects as S-; Stage 4: other fish species as S-. Stage 2 was repeated after stages 3 (step 2) or 4 (step 3) for the individuals that did not reach the learning criterion

Table 1 Set of all the stimuli used in stages 1–4

If the maximum number of sessions was reached by the subject without validating the learning criteria, we repeated stage 2 (black shape as S-) – the initial successful stage of the paradigm – as a control to ensure that the overall performance of the trained 2-AFC task did not decline with time or difficulty, as shown in other studies (archerfish: Newport, Wallis, Temple, & Siebeck, 2013). Therefore, we used 3 steps for stage 2 (Fig. 2). To prevent any side bias in the first learning exercise (stage 2), a correction trial (CT) was implemented in the case of an incorrect response using the same procedure as described in Knolle, Goncalves, and Morton (2017). This allowed reusing the exact same set of stimuli in the experiment until a correct response was made. However, in subsequent stages, no CT was performed since these stages (objects and other species as S-) were not training stages but testing stages with increasing difficulty. Thus, if a side bias was observed in stages 3 and 4, only a forced test was conducted to eliminate it. This test consisted of a unique S+ presentation (one of the four photographs of conspecifics randomly presented) on the screen on the side opposite from the preferred side until the fish chose the initially non-preferred side in three consecutive trials. No S- was presented in the forced test. From stage 2, the triggers were raised so that they were immersed only 5.5 cm below the water surface (instead of 9 cm) to prevent involuntary activations.

Stimuli

All stimuli were two-dimensional (2-D) pictures created using Microsoft PowerPoint and ImageJ (Table 1). Images used as positive stimuli were photographs of four juvenile female triploid rainbow trout unfamiliar to the tested trout. The different S+ images used in pretraining (different shapes of different colors) and the S- images (four blue shapes and a black star) used in stages 1 and 2 were created using PowerPoint. During pretraining, 26 different shapes of nine different colors were used randomly, but fish were never exposed to images of fish, objects, or black shapes. For stages 3 and 4, the images used as negative stimuli were all taken from the internet. Stimuli were all stretched according to the PowerPoint slide size (25.4 × 19.05 cm) and were individually saved in png format. Then, a macro from ImageJ was run to adjust their brightness. On the screen, each stimulus appeared at a size of 14.5 × 11 cm.

Statistical analyses

All tests and graphs were performed using the 1.2.1335 version of RStudio and were plotted using the packages ggplot2, ggthemes, ggsignif, and effects.

Daily discrimination rates shown by each subject are described graphically as the percent of correct responses per session.

Learning criteria were considered significantly different from chance by using binomial tests. The chance level was 50% in the case of two triggers.

The percentages of correct responses for each trout during the first session and last session (within each stage) were compared with paired Wilcoxon nonparametric tests.

Eventual preference (proportion of correct responses) for any S+ (four images of trout) and any S- (60 images of objects and 60 of other fish species) was analyzed by using generalized linear models (GLMs) with the MASS package (in R 3.5.3), considering a binomial distribution. The following general linear model was used: glm (cbind(Nb.successes,Nb.failures) ~ Image, family=binomial (link=logit)).

Eventual effect of trout running order (the order fish were netted to perform a session) on the percentage of correct responses was analyzed using linear models (LMs) with the car package. The following LM was used: lm(Score ~ Running.order).

The results of the GLM and LM analyses were then obtained thanks to the analysis of deviance table with ANOVA (chi-square tests for the binomial family). When significant effects were found, post hoc analyses were performed using HSD-Tukey tests.

Paired Wilcoxon tests were used to check whether there was global lateralization (percent of right vs. left activations) within each 2-AFC stage conducted.

P-values < 0.05 were considered statistically significant for all statistical analyses.

Results

Training to activate triggers

Nine of the 15 trout initially trained to activate the triggers reached the learning criterion of 70% correct responses in three consecutive sessions (binomial: P < 0.001, N = 30 trials) after three to 15 sessions. The trout that reached the criterion after only three sessions is the one that unfortunately died during the experiment and is thus not further included in the results, which take into account only the five remaining fish. One fish never activated a trigger. The others sometimes activated a trigger, but none were able to reach an accuracy above the level of chance.

Pretraining to activate a trigger associated with a stimulus

After a total of 540 trials, none of the nine fish reached the learning criterion of 80% in two consecutive sessions when using shapes of different colors as S+ (binomial: P > 0.05, N = 20 trials).

2-AFC with photographs of conspecifics as S+

For all stages, no differences in selection frequency were found among the four different conspecific photographs presented as S+ when analyzing the different linear models (GLMs: P > 0.05). Similarly, no preference or aversion was found for any images of objects (stage 3) or other fish species (stage 4) presented as S- (P > 0.05). We did not find any (right or left) side bias at any stage (Wilcoxon: V = 10, P > 0.05).

Stage 1: Blue shapes as S-

The first stage of the 2-AFC test consisted of displaying four blue shapes as S- and four recurrent photographs of conspecifics as S+. Even though the scores were mainly at or above the level of chance, none of the five individuals were able to meet the learning criterion in 30 completed sessions (Fig. 3). Trout No. 2 died before completing the 30 sessions and we thus replaced it in the remaining stages by Trout No. 2 bis, after it succeeded in the habituation phase (Table 2).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Number of sessions needed to complete each stage by each individual (Nos. 1–5). The red labels indicate that the stage was not completed successfully

Table 2 Percentages of correct responses obtained by the five trout per session for each stage of the two-alternative forced-choice test task task

Stage 2: Black shape as S-

The learning criterion of 75% in two consecutive sessions was reached by the five tested individuals (stage 2-step 1, Fig. 3). The trout needed between seven and 14 sessions (84–168 trials) to reach the criterion (Table 2).

Only trout No. 4 was subjected to stage 2-step 2 since it failed in stage 3 (objects as S-). The learning criterion was again reached by this trout after nine sessions (P = 0.006, N = 24 trials) (Fig. 3). The five individuals passed stage 2-step 3 since they all failed in stage 4 (species as S-), and they all again met the learning criterion in two to 15 sessions, except trout No. 5 (maximum of 30 sessions reached).

Stage 3: Objects as S-

When displaying objects as distracting stimuli, four individuals (out of five) reached a statistically significant S+ selection frequency (≥ 75% correct responses in two consecutive sessions) in two to 12 sessions (binomial: P < 0.01, N = 24 trials) (Fig. 3, Table 2). Trout No. 4 reached the maximum number of 30 sessions without meeting the learning criterion. This trout reached the 75% correct response level only once (session 13) (Table 2).

Stage 4: Other fish species as S-

None of the five individuals was able to reach the learning criterion within the maximum of ten sessions established for this stage (Fig. 3). Most of the scores were at or below chance level (Table 2). Trout No. 1 twice reached 75% or more correct responses but in nonconsecutive sessions (sessions 3 and 9), and trout No. 4 met the criterion only once (session 8) (Table 2).

Comparison between first and last session within each stage

We compared the percentage of correct responses between the first session and the last session for each stage for the five trout tested in the 2-AFC task (Fig. 4). The results indicate a significant increase between the first and last sessions for the trigger habituation stage (Wilcoxon: V = 0, P = 0.031, N = 5 individuals), for stage 2 (trout as S+ and black shape as S-), step 1 (V = 0, P = 0.029) and for stage 3 (trout as S+ and objects as S-) (V = 0, P = 0.029). No differences were found for stage 1 (trout as S+ and blue shapes as S-) (V = 9, P = 0.812) and stage 4 (trout as S+ and other fish species as S-) (V = 8, P = 0.607).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Median (quartiles: 25%, 75%) percentages of correct responses during the first and last sessions for each stage: habituation to trigger activation, blue shapes as S- (stage 1), black shape as S- (stage 2, only step 1 is presented), objects as S- (stage 3) and other fish species as S- (stage 4). A photograph of a trout is presented as S+ in each two-alternative forced-choice test task test. Each color is dedicated to a stage, and dots represent the proportion of correct responses provided by the five individuals. The black dotted line represents chance level. Medians were compared between the first and last sessions using the Wilcoxon test (*: P<0.05; ns: nonsignificant; N = 5)

We found a significant effect of fish running order on the percentage of correct responses for stage 4 only (LM: F4,45 = 2.90, P < 0.05). Fish netted in fifth position had poorer results than those netted in the first (Tukey test: P = 0.027) and third positions (P = 0.017).

Discussion

Here, we investigated the capacity of rainbow trout to solve a two-choice discrimination task by using an operant conditioning device. Fish used the operant conditioning apparatus successfully, and we showed that they were able to discriminate between recurrent 2-D photographs of conspecifics and either images of a black shape or any objects displayed on a screen but not between photographs of conspecifics and blue shapes or images of other fish species.

Success and usefulness of the operant conditioning device

In the habituation phase, fish were required to manipulate a trigger to obtain a food reward while being isolated in a novel tank. Nine individuals out of 15 completed this task and required between three and 15 sessions (ten trials each). This result coincides with that obtained for the African cichlid Pseudotropheus sp., which needed approximately 100 ± 47 trials (10 ± 4.7 sessions) to get used to a 2-AFC device (approach the food holder to obtain a reward) (Schluessel et al., 2012). This performance is impressive in our case considering that the rainbow trout faced difficulties regarding the initial group-rearing conditions and ecology. Indeed, the first challenge might have been to become acclimatized alone in an unknown area, which represents a considerable source of stress for rainbow trout reared in groups. This situation resembles the novel-tank test commonly used to experimentally evaluate emotional reactivity of fish (Champagne, Hoefnagels, de Kloet, & Richardson, 2010; Colson et al., 2015). Time was therefore necessary for acclimation and to associate netting and the novel tank with the positive event (highly appetitive food distribution). Then, the trout were required to learn how to activate a self-feeder individually outside of their initial context of group use. Trout reared in groups of 100–300 individuals require approximately 25 days to reach a stable level of self-feeding (Alanara & Brannas, 1996). The trout therefore learned to handle a self-feeder trigger faster on their own – while being isolated in the test tank – than in a group since it took them 90 ± 60 trials (9 ± 6 sessions). The use of an operant conditioning device with self-feeders positioned in front of corresponding visual stimuli represents in itself a significant performance because it involves a complete panel of cognitive abilities at the same time (spatial orientation, lever manipulation, object recognition, temporal and spatial association of environmental cues) (Jurado-Parras et al., 2013). To our knowledge, this is the first study demonstrating such cognitive performance in a farmed-fish species. Studies generally use discrete operant responses, such as approaching sensors (Siebeck et al., 2009), time spent near stimuli (Kuroda et al., 2017), passing through a door to enter a rewarded chamber (Bloch et al., 2019; Gomez-Laplaza et al, 2018; Mueller & Neuhauss, 2012), but none where fish had to operate a lever as an active discrete conditioned response. With this last category of conditioned response, we demonstrated complex and complete cognitive abilities in rainbow trout. Two reasons may explain their rapid utilization of this operant conditioning device. First, trout were reared in enriched tanks (containing artificial plants, rocks, and pipes), environmental enrichment being known to enhance fish cognitive capacity (Salvanes et al., 2013; Strand et al., 2010). However, the effect of environmental enrichment facilitating operant conditioning in fish deserves to be specifically tested in future studies. Secondly, the experimentation began when the trout were juveniles, a life stage with an important behavioral plasticity (Kotrschal & Taborsky, 2010).

While the majority of fish studies requires the intervention of the experimenter between trials to correctly process the test (except in Brock, Sudwarts, Daggett, Parker, & Brennan, 2017; Mueller & Neuhauss, 2012), the present testing device was fully automated to prevent any fish disturbance. To measure behavior, automating procedures are commonly used in non-human primates and birds where subjects are required to make an active choice with touchscreen technologies and increase the reliability and standardization (within and between laboratory) (Brock et al., 2017). Our device brings for the first time in fish a reliable automated tool closer to the those used in other species, increasing both the flexibility and translational relevance of the results (Horner et al., 2013). This setup can be added to the existing repertoire of testing devices suitable for the investigation of simple (decision-making, object recognition) to more complex (working memory, numerical rules, categorization) cognitive abilities in fish.

Forced-choice test: One stimulus

The major challenge in the pretraining procedure was to create an association between a visual stimulus positioned behind one trigger (out of two) and the activation of the respective trigger, which only then delivered food. Here, the rewarded stimulus was different shapes of different colors that changed between trials. After a total of 540 trials, no fish reached the learning criterion. Previous studies report that cichlids required 40 ± 20 trials (Schluessel et al., 2012), horses required between 840 and 1,100 trials (Gabor & Gerken, 2012), and goats needed approximately 1,000 trials (Langbein et al., 2004) to learn a similar task but using only one or two different black symbols. The presentation of only one similar stimulus (or two) may have helped the fish to focus on the correct trigger and reach the learning criterion more easily in this task. However, a recent study showed that zebrafish were able to make a 2-AFC task by moving directly from the habituation stage to the 2-AFC paradigm without any presentation of a unique stimulus (Bloch et al., 2019). Therefore, pretraining using only one stimulus displayed on the screen may not have been necessary in our experimental design. Indeed, when the 2-AFC test began with a photograph of a conspecific as S+ and a distracting stimulus (S-) displayed on either side, fish started to respond above chance level. As predicted by Fuss, Bleckmann, and Schluessel (2014), they seemed more attracted by a biologically relevant stimulus than to any kind of abstract shape. They also possibly needed the presence of a distracting stimulus, allowing the possibility to show a relative preference for the reinforced image of fish. Similarly, sharks need twice as many sessions to discriminate between a positive stimulus and a blank screen than between two distinct stimuli (Fuss et al., 2014). This is in accordance with Catania (1975), who demonstrated a preference for the availability of alternatives over unique stimuli in pigeons exposed to choice tests.

Alternative forced-choice test: Two stimuli

When the 2-AFC started, the distracting stimuli were represented by four blue shapes (and images of conspecifics as S+), and no fish performed successfully within 30 sessions. Blue has been shown to be one of the preferred colors of rainbow trout (along with green) when given a choice (Luchiari & Pirhonen, 2008; Maia et al., 2017). This visual preference for blue color may have caused a conflict of interest between the two stimuli presented on the screen, and affected the discrimination performance, as observed in zebrafish (Oliveira et al., 2015; Roy et al., 2019). Moreover, blue shapes were also presented as S+ in the pretraining procedure and could have acted as a reversal instruction, increasing the conflict of interest between blue shapes as S- and images of conspecifics as S+. To increase the visual contrast between stimuli and because other species succeeded when using only one or two shapes as S- for a similar study design (Gabor & Gerken, 2012; Langbein et al., 2004; Schluessel et al., 2012), a unique black shape was ultimately chosen as S- in stage 2. This constituted a success since all the fish tested with this last combination met the learning criterion in a range of 126 ± 42 trials (10.5 ± 3.5 sessions). Therefore, when given the appropriate stimuli, rainbow trout are able to learn a 2-AFC task as quickly as higher-order vertebrates (horses: Gabor & Gerken, 2012; goats: Langbein et al., 2004). Similar cognitive performance has also been demonstrated in other fish species. Cichlids took 116 ± 90 trials (11.6 ± 9 sessions) and goldfish took 2,000–3,500 trials (20–35 sessions) to discriminate between a square and a triangle (Schluessel et al., 2012). The gray bamboo shark needed 98 ± 50 trials (9.75 ± 5.04 sessions) (Fuss et al., 2014) to reach a 75% correct response level in the same task (discriminating a square from a triangle). In a 2-AFC test based on color discrimination (red and green), five sessions were sufficient for zebrafish to meet the learning criterion (Bloch et al., 2019). Here, we show for the first time similar discriminative abilities between two different visual stimuli (a black star and photographs of conspecifics) in a selected farmed fish species – while other studies used principally wild fish or fish from pet stores.

The fish decision strategy could have been to avoid S- on the basis of its characteristics instead of associating the photograph of a conspecific with a reward, as previously observed in Peters’ elephant-nose fish (von der Emde & Fetz, 2007). However, four individuals (out of five) were successful in discriminating between images of conspecifics and objects in 7 ± 5 sessions in stage 3. This indicates that the fish did not simply learn to avoid the black star in the previous stage but rather chose the S+ because of its positive association with the reward. We cannot exclude that an innate visual preference for photographs of conspecifics has explained this result. These preferences can be very strong and affect discrimination performance, as shown in other fish species (Oliveira et al., 2015; Roy et al., 2019). However, the fact that performance significantly changed between the first and the last sessions suggests the role of learning here. Also, trout failed to reach the learning criterion in stage 1 and stage 4, which suggests that they have no innate preference for the photographs of conspecifics presented as S+. To validate this finding, it would be interesting to use the photographs used here as S+ as the S- in a future study. The individual that did not succeed in this task was tested again in stage 2 to control for its overall performance in the 2-AFC task, and it discriminated again between the black star and the images of conspecifics in nine sessions. This confirms that this individual did not stop being motivated to use the apparatus but was unable to perform correctly in stage 3, illustrating the intraspecific variability in behavioral responses existing in rainbow trout. We noticed that this individual was easily stressed, and any perturbation (here, a novel stage) was sufficient to induce behavioral inhibition, resulting in the stress-coping style described in highly responsive strains of rainbow trout (Overli et al., 2007). The 60 objects used as S- in stage 3 were all taken from the internet and were as various as a clock, an armchair, a computer, a desk, and a bottle. No object was particularly preferred/more often chosen by the fish during all the sessions, and the only common feature among them was their nonbiological relevance for fish. Schluessel et al. (2012) showed that the African cichlid Pseudotropheus sp. distinguished between two mental categories (“fish” and “snail”) by examining the entire stimulus rather than individual features. The authors consider that since predators, prey, conspecifics, and objects (shelters or landmarks) present in a habitat may vary in size, shape, and color, using general similar features provides fish with a certain degree of flexibility to react appropriately. Here, fish might have distinctly memorized or classified the two categories “objects” and “fish” in their entirety as nonbiologically relevant and biologically relevant, or non-fish and fish, respectively.

When discrimination difficulty increased in stage 4, where photographs of other fish species were displayed as S- (and conspecifics as S+), performances dropped to the chance threshold. In this case, the visual difference between S- and S+ was not as obvious as that between a fish and an object, and all individuals failed in the two-discrimination task. However, we know that teleost fish are able to see complex and contrasting patterns (Ben-Simon et al., 2012; Champ et al., 2014; Gierszewski et al., 2013), but note that none of these studies have specifically focused on the visual acuity of rainbow trout. Rainbow trout recognize visually their conspecifics (Höjesjö et al., 2015; Johnsson & Åkerman, 1998), but no study investigated whether rainbow trout can discriminate them from heterospecifics. Here, we considered that the fish recognized the general outline of a conspecific as a fish but similarly recognized the shape of any other fish species. If trout learned the category "fish" (all species combined because of biological relevance) rather than remembering the four photographs of their conspecifics, this could explain the poor performance observed in stage 4, linked to the shared features between the positive and the negative stimulus. However, three scores reached the 75% level of correct responses but in nonconsecutive sessions. A maximum of ten sessions was chosen for this stage since the instruction given (discriminating between two images of fish) was considered to be too ambiguous and more than ten sessions would have perhaps cancelled the initial positive association of an image of a trout with a reward and could have acted as a reversal learning, as shown in zebrafish (Kuroda et al., 2017). Therefore, perhaps a few more sessions would have been necessary to know with accuracy whether rainbow trout are able to visually distinguish between conspecifics and other fish species in this experimental set up. An extra experiment running stage 4 for more than ten sessions is necessary and is currently planned to elucidate this question. Another explanation for this failure in stage 4 concerns the use of static images displayed on screen monitors in the investigation of live animal discrimination. Many fish species – including rainbow trout (Höjesjö et al., 2015; Johnsson & Åkerman, 1998) – are able to recognize live conspecifics individually using visual cues (Culumber, 2015; Griffiths & Magurran, 1999; Roux et al., 2016). However, when males and females of the peacock blenny (Salaria pavo) were presented with a live conspecific male or a video of the same individual, the video images failed to elicit appropriate behavioral responses (Goncalves, Oliveira, Korner, Poschadel, & Schlupp, 2000). In the case of anemonefishes (Johnston & Dixson, 2017) and Lake Malawi cichlid species (Knight & Turner, 1999), it was shown that a combination of visual and other senses stimuli (chemical cues) can be fundamental in conspecific recognition. The importance of olfactory versus visual cues in con- and hetero-specifics recognition varies across species (Santacà, Busatta, Lucon-Xiccato & Bisazza, 2019). As in other fish species (coral reef damselfish: Holmes & McCormick, 2010; zebrafish: Speedie & Gerlai, 2008; cichlids: Vavrek & Brown, 2009), rainbow trout can perceive conspecifics using chemical cues (Brown & Smith, 1997). In the motionless 2-D images displayed on the screen, rainbow trout could perceive neither behavioral features nor other sensory cues (olfaction or mechanoreception) present in live fish. In this way, 2-D images may not be appropriate for rainbow trout to recognize between con- and hetero-specifics.

Interestingly, an effect of trout’s running order was observed only at this stage. An individual tested at the last place (fifth) presented weaker performances than when tested earlier. One could have predicted the opposite result, fish being tested earlier in the day (closer to their last feeding) being less motivated to receive the food-reward than others. However, the overnight food-deprivation period varied between fish from 18 h to 24 h (depending on their running order), which is a duration long enough for all of them to ensure a similar food motivation. More importantly, when a stage appeared easier to the fish (stages 2 and 3), running order did not affect the conditioned responses. The time of passage possibly interfered with the stress accumulated by repeated netting in the rearing tank, affecting the score of the last fish netted at stage 4 only, when the instruction given required all fish attention. Indeed, acute stress is known to impair fish attention and learning performance (Gaikwad et al., 2011). To control for their overall performance in the 2-AFC task, the black star was again presented as S-, and four individuals (out of five) regained the criterion level in stage 2-step 3. Again, the failure to discriminate between two images of fish (con- and hetero-specifics) was thus not simply due to a lack of motivation to answer.

In training procedures, the success of a few individuals is sufficient to generalize proven cognitive abilities to the whole species. In some cases, even one individual completing a task is adequate, meaning that the brain of the members of the species is endowed with neurocognitive systems that likely support the resolution of the task (Agrillo & Bisazza, 2014). As in the present experiment, animal cognitive studies generally include few experimental subjects (archerfish: six individuals tested: Newport, Wallis, & Siebeck, 2015; sheep: eight individuals tested: Knolle et al., 2017; horses: seven individuals tested: Gabor & Gerken, 2012). We can therefore consider that in our experimental paradigm, rainbow trout species are capable of discriminating in a 2-AFC task between a photograph of conspecifics and a black star (five individuals succeeding) and between conspecifics and any other object (four individuals succeeding). This finding adds to the body of evidence that fish have good learning skills, even in the absence of a neocortex, the cerebral region involved in learning processes in mammals (Seger & Miller, 2010), and presents a novel method with which to study a diverse range of fish discrimination abilities.

Rainbow trout cognitive performance under farming conditions

The present study demonstrates that a farmed fish species is capable of object recognition and likely possesses categorization learning abilities. Distinguishing items in the environment – i.e., conspecifics and objects (pipes or landmarks) present in the rearing tank – in an organized manner (categories) allows fish to solve problems, communicate in groups, and obtain information (Oliveira et al., 2015). Their ability to visually recognize different items present in the environment and to memorize them might reduce costly efforts to consider as a new information the same item when encountered again. Individual recognition may even help to decrease aggression by ensuring a stable social hierarchy since trout avoid repeating costly social interactions when they have already confronted each other’s (Johnsson, 1997; Johnsson & Akerman, 1998). Finally, distinguishing those items without ambiguity certainly allows rainbow trout to spend time on other essential activities (foraging, exploring) and reduce stress due to the inherent unpredictability of captive environments.

The performance exhibited by rainbow trout while using the conditioned device opens new perspectives concerning the possible cognitive enrichments to be developed in future farming systems. Cognitive enrichment, also known as occupational or psychological enrichment, represents the possibility for animals to meet moderate challenges by using their cognitive abilities and/or to actively interact with their environment (Manteuffel et al., 2009; Meehan & Mench, 2007; Oesterwind et al., 2016). Challenges range from simple manipulations (devices to obtain food) to more complex challenges (puzzle solving) tailored to the ecology of the species. It mainly aims at reducing psychological monotony of often-impoverished environments by introducing predictable and/or animal-controllable variations (Naslund & Johnsson, 2016). Reducing the psychological monotony of impoverished environments might become more important after highlighting the complex cognitive performance displayed by rainbow trout through this experiment. This knowledge could be applied to provide stimulations for engaging their advanced cognitive capacities, in order to improve their rearing conditions. For instance, novel cognitive enrichments could include visual stimulations inside the tanks or “improved” self-feeders (delivering food only when a goal is achieved), and would enhance fish welfare under future farming conditions.

These findings are also important in a welfare context since evidence that farm animals have cognitive skills comparable to those of so-called higher vertebrates encourages social awareness of the need to improve their welfare.

Conclusions

To conclude, rainbow trout rapidly learned how to operate a self-feeder individually in conditions of social isolation. This reliable automated tool closer to the operant setup used in higher species can be added to the existing repertoire of testing devices suitable for the investigation of cognitive abilities in fish. We also showed that rainbow trout are able to discriminate a pool of four recurrent conspecifics from a unique black shape and from different objects but not from other fish species. Whether the trout needed more than ten trials to demonstrate their ability to discriminate visually between con- and hetero-specifics, if they learned a category “fish shape” or “biologically relevant” and thus could not distinguish between the positive and negative stimuli presented because of shared features, or if they instead needed to see the two “fish” stimuli live to perceive any behavioral or other sensory features, still needs to be further tested in dedicated experiments. With the visual items chosen for the 2-AFC task, we cannot completely exclude that at some of the results were due to innate visual preferences for the photographs of conspecifics, but the fact that the performance increased between the first and the last session suggests the role of learning here. The ability to visually recognize different items present in the environment and to memorize them reduces costly efforts to consider as new information the same item when encountered again.

This study sheds new light on the mental capacities of rainbow trout, a farmed animal often forgotten in public concerns. Its success in operating self-feeders in a complex situation may help us to develop species-specific ethological-based tools, or cognitive enrichments, in order to increase the welfare of the fish in future farming systems.