Introduction

The role that technology plays in modern life continues to provoke debate in academic literature and across popular media (e.g., Stieger & Lewetz, 2018; Twenge, 2017). Social media remains core to many of these concerns as it has radically changed the way we socialize. For example, social networking sites and online messaging services, such as Facebook, present opportunities for users to instantly share social information with large numbers of people (Subrahmanyam, Reich, Waechter, & Espinoza, 2008). These activities are elementary to the basic human need for obtaining social information and relatedness (Day, Ong, & Perry, 2018), and avoiding loneliness and isolation (Martín-Albo et al., 2015). Indeed, engaging with some elements of social media has been found to improve psychosocial well-being (Chan, 2018a, 2018b; Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007; Obst & Stafurik, 2010; Wang, Brede, Ianni, & Mentzakis, 2018) and frequent social media use has been related to reduced loneliness, through users’ feelings of satisfaction with their connections to friends (Deters & Mehl, 2013; Sheldon, Abad, & Hinsch, 2011). Conversely, a large body of research demonstrates associations between heightened online social information seeking, known as ‘social media addiction’, and negative outcomes including low self-esteem and life satisfaction (Andreassen, Pallesen, & Griffiths, 2017; Błachnio, Przepiorka, & Pantic, 2016). Further, those who report higher levels of loneliness also estimate increased Facebook use (Błachnio & Przepiorka, 2019). Establishing causality remains challenging, however. While Facebook may increase loneliness following social comparisons, people could simply seek more social information as a form of comfort (Wang et al., 2018).

This research overwhelmingly reports high rates of social media ‘addiction’ (up to 34% of some samples, see review by Andreassen, 2015). This reported rate of addiction is cause for concern, especially as this might be reflected in other social behaviors. For example, individuals frequently seek social information in offline contexts. Increased offline social time comes with risks, too, as face-to-face social pressure can lead to increased risk-taking behavior, such as in ‘peer pressure’ situations (Gheorghiu, Delhomme, & Felonneau, 2015; Lewis & Lewis, 1984). With the danger of increased offline social behavior and the measurement practices in social media addiction research, we considered how we could adopt similar approaches to measuring behavioral addiction to evaluate ‘offline friend addiction’. In doing so, we assess the prevalence of addictive behaviors in an alternative social context, which might be driven by similar parallel psychological processes. For example, self-determination theory highlights that individuals’ well-being is associated with their relatedness with others (Deci & Ryan, 1985), and this is correlates with daily mood, whereby increased seeking of social contact aligns with more positive mood (Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan, 2000). Research has shown that social media use is also relevant for relatedness (Lin, 2016), and it could be the case that spending time with friends offline meets this need as well. While it is the case that there are psychological traits that influence a need to seek social information from others, to our knowledge, no social media addiction research has accounted for the role of offline friend addiction behavior.

Here, we follow the social media addiction literature closely to create and validate the Offline-Friend Addiction Questionnaire (O-FAQ) by re-wording existing measures. For example, the Social Media Craving Scale was previously adapted from the Penn Alcohol Craving Scale (Savci & Griffiths, 2019) and the Facebook Intrusion Questionnaire from the Mobile Phone Involvement Questionnaire (Elphinston & Noller, 2011). While we do not currently have a psychiatric (medical) criteria to validate our scale, we can use self-reported personality traits (see Andreassen, Torsheim, Brunborg, & Pallesen, 2012; Orosz, Tóth-Király, & Bőthe, 2016), self-reported problematic behaviors (see Andreassen et al., 2012; Savci & Griffiths, 2019), and self-reported estimates of spent socially interacting (Elphinston & Noller, 2011; Orosz et al., 2016; Savci & Griffiths, 2019) as proxies for our target construct. We can also use our validation measures of risk-taking behavior to highlight the dangers of offline social behavior. Following the practice in the literature, we also aim to categorize participants as being addicted to spending time with their friends. To do this, we follow the frequently cited ‘polythetic’ classifying criteria from Adreasson et al. (Andreassen et al., 2012) to define an addict as an individual who scores ‘neither agree nor disagree’ on one more than half of the items in our questionnaire (examples following this guidance include Brailovskaia, Schillack, & Margraf, 2018; Brailovskaia, Margraf, & Köllner, 2019; Gul, Yurumez Solmaz, Gul, & Oner, 2018; Hou, Xiong, Jiang, Song, & Wang, 2019; Jafarkarimi, Sim, Saadatdoost, & Hee, 2016; Rajesh & Rangaiah, 2020; Sampath, Kalyani, Soohinda, & Dutta, 2017).

The aim of this study (as declared in our pre-registration: https://osf.io/9y2rh/) is that by following the typical methods of social media addiction research, we will be able to develop, validate, and establish reliability of a scale that categorizes individuals as addicted to their friends.

Method

Our aims, sample size, procedure, and analysis choices are detailed in our Open Science Framework (OSF) pre-registration, which can be found here: https://osf.io/9y2rh/. This study received ethical approval from the University of Derby and the study was conducted to the standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and the British Psychological Society.

Participants

This multi-lab study recruited an opportunity sample of undergraduate students from 11 different universities across the U.K. using participant pools and online advertising. Participants were eligible if they were at least 18 years of age and fluent in English. Our recruitment advert requested that individuals diagnosed with a mental health condition (such as bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, or any condition that has required hospitalization over the last 12 months, not including depression or anxiety) did not take part in the study. Our data collection stopping rule (see preregistration) was to collect enough data so that we could adequately analyze test–retest data from at least 320 participants who had completed Time 1 and Time 2 responses (to detect a minimum correlation of r = .20, assuming α and β of .05). We stopped Time 1 recruitment when we had collected N = 328 complete Time 2 responses. We then waited 28 days for the last of the Time 2 reports.

At Time 1, 1039 individuals engaged with the survey link. Of these, N = 807 participants completed all Time 1 measures (nFemale = 640, nMale = 161, MAge = 28.36, SDAge = 11.68) and their data was retained for analysis. The majority of these participants were from the UK (nUK = 593).

At Time 1, participants could elect to provide their e-mail address for a test–retest 28 days after their initial submission. After removing the participants whose Time 2 identifying codes did not match any Time 1 codes, N = 313 (nFemale = 250, nMale = 58, MAge = 28.93, SDAge = 12.54, nUK = 243) participants remained for test–retest analysis.

Materials

Our full questionnaire, with all questions as presented to participants, can be found in our OSF materials.

  • Offline-Friend Addiction Questionnaire (O-FAQ). The O-FAQ was adapted from previous measures of pathological social media use. We reframed established measures’ items to assess our target construct. We adapted the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (Andreassen et al., 2012; cited by 969 at time of writing), the Facebook Instruction Scale (Elphinston & Noller, 2011; cited by 472), the Facebook Intensity ScaleFootnote 1 (Orosz et al., 2016; cited by 52), and the most recent Social Media Craving Scale (Savci & Griffiths, 2019) by changing references to ‘Facebook’ or ‘Social Media’ to ‘spending time with friends’. Some items required additional adaptation to be suitable (most notably the Social Media Craving Scale’s items, which use a different response format). The complete O-FAQ had 37 items and is presented in Table 1. This measure was responded to on a scale of Strongly Disagree (1) to Strongly Agree (5).

  • Validation measures. In line with the aims of the study, we used social media addiction literature precedent to validate the O-FAQ. In all cases, aggregate factor scores of existing measures were retained for analysis.

Table 1 Sources, descriptive statistics, and exploratory factor analysis loadings from exploratory factor analysis of the Offline-Friend Addiction Questionnaire

To demonstrate that the O-FAQ shares variance with other measures of social preference and anxiety), we examined the relationship between the O-FAQ and the Big Five personality traits (as per Andreassen et al., 2012; Orosz et al., 2016). We used the Big Five Inventory (John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008) which showed good internal reliability in our Time 1 data for Conscientiousness (α = .79, MC = 3.52, SDC = 0.64), Agreeableness (α = .74, MA = 3.84, SDA = 0.61), Neuroticism (α = .85, MN = 3.32, SDN = 0.82), Openness (α = .77, MO = 3.53, SDO = 0.61), and Extraversion (α = .87, ME = 3.16, SDE = 0.85).

To demonstrate convergent validity with other measures of problematic behavior (as per Andreassen et al., 2012; Savci & Griffiths, 2019), we included a measure of Domain Specific Risk Taking (DOSPERT; Blais & Weber, 2006). The DOSPERT contains 30-items assessing different types of risk-taking. At Time 1, the reliability of the DOSPERT items were on the lower end of acceptable for Social (α = .63, M = 5.01, SD = 0.99), Recreational (α = .80, M = 3.22, SD = 1.48), Ethical (α = .60, M = 2.18, SD = 0.87), Financial (α = .75, M = 2.22, SD = 0.99), and Health (α = .64, M = 3.16, SD =1.18) risk-taking.

To demonstrate that the scale is measuring the engagement with the behavior in question, we were guided by previous research (Elphinston & Noller, 2011; Orosz et al., 2016; Savci & Griffiths, 2019) and asked “In the past 7 days, how many hours have you spent with friends (in person)?” At Time 1, participants reported spending on average 18.36 hours with friends in person (SDw/friends1 = 22.34) and 16.33 hours at Time 2 (SDw/friends2 = 21.06). Individuals were notably consistent in their amount of in-person friend contact across the four-week period (α = .77, d = 0.09).

Procedure

Participants were recruited for an online questionnaire on ‘personality and friendship’. After consenting, respondents provided demographic information (age, gender, home country) and how much time they spent with their friends over the last week (in person). Participants then completed the O-FAQ, then the Big Five measure, and finally the DOSPERT. They also created a unique identifying code for the confidential matching of their Time 1 and Time 2 data and provided an e-mail address to be contacted for a follow-up.

At Time 2 participants were asked how much time they spent with their friends over the last week (in person) and completed the O-FAQ for a second time.

Analytical Strategy

All analysis was conducted in R, and our code is available on the OSF (https://osf.io/9y2rh/).

  • Inference criteria. Our analytical choices are all stated a priori in our preregistration. Following the precedent for drawing inference in previous similar research, the results will be evaluated in null hypothesis tests (with ɑ = .05), and we report the 95% CI of effect sizes.

  • Factor analysis criteria. We used exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to evaluate the structure of the O-FAQ. This EFA was conducted after reaching our pre-registered criterion of n > 300 completions at Time 1, with the aim of producing a short-form measure within the 28 days before Time 2. In line with previous research, we built a direct oblimin EFA (Elphinston & Noller, 2011), and tested the data's suitability for EFA using Bartlett's sphericity test (accepting data for EFA where p < .05; Savci & Griffiths, 2019). We then tested the acceptability of a one-factor solution (as others have done with social media addiction measures; Andreassen et al., 2012). If a one-factor solution was found to be adequate (see Table 2; Savci & Griffiths, 2019), we pre-registered that this model would be accepted.

  • Validation analyses. In line with our strategy to closely follow previous literature, we demonstrate the validity of the O-FAQ with; correlations between the O-FAQ and the Big Five traits (similarly to Andreasson et al., 2012; Orosz et al., 2016), correlations between the O-FAQ and the amount of time participants report spending with friends (in hours) (see Elphinston & Noller, 2011), and correlations between the O-FAQ and problematic behavior (the DOSPERT; see Savci & Griffiths, 2019). In line with previous work (Andreassen et al., 2012), we also conduct two step regressions predicting the O-FAQ domains with age, gender of participants, and time spent with friends in Step 1 and then Step 2 including the personality and risk-taking traits.

  • Consistency analysis. To demonstrate test–retest consistency, we conducted correlations between Time 1 and Time 2 O-FAQ domain scores.

  • ‘Addict’ classification. Following previous literature, we categorically define 'addicts' in our sample. We will do this using the polythetic and monothetic criteria used by others. Specifically, we follow frequently cited guidance provided by Andreassen et al. (2012, p. 512), which defines a ‘polythetic scoring scheme’ as scoring 3 or above [out of 5] on at least four of the six items” and a ‘monothetic scoring key’ as “(e.g., scoring 3 [out of 5] or above on all six items)”. In our study, we will classify participants as addicts on the 'conservative' monothetic criteria of responding "neither agree nor disagree (3)" or higher (≥ 3) on all items (K). Further, we will classify participants as addicts on the 'liberal' polythetic criteria of responding "neither agree nor disagree (3)" or higher (≥ 3) on the majority ((K/2)+1) of items.

Table 2 The model fit criteria for the O-FAQ item reduction

Results

Structure of Offline-Friend Addiction Questionnaire (O-FAQ)

Before the 28-day cut-off point for our attempted item reduction, we had collected n = 421 responses to the O-FAQ (nFemale = 319, nMale = 99, MAge = 32.71, SDAge = 12.37, nUK = 273). We conducted an oblimin EFA on data suitable for factor analysis (Bartlett's sphericity test χ2 = 7151.64, p < .001). We sought a one-factor solution for our data, which was not an adequate fit based on our preregistered criteria taken from Savci and Griffiths (2019), see Table 2. The closest fit to all the precedent-defined acceptable criteria was a seven-factor solution. This would create multiple small factors with few items and was not considered an efficient solution to the factor analysis. Therefore, we chose to deviate from our preregistration criteria and adopted a three-factor solution which met the majority of the acceptability criteria (except TLI; Table 2). For item loadings, see Table 1. There were no items that were low-loading by the criteria we established in our preregistration section (< .30) and so we did not reduce the number of items in the O-FAQ for Time 2 assessment.

The first factor, labeled Social Rumination, contained 20 items reflecting preoccupied thoughts about spending time with others and using time with friends to avoid boredom. Items in this factor included; “I often think about the times I've spent with friends”, “If I could do only one thing in a day, it would be to spend time with friends” and “At the most challenging point of last week, I have a strong craving to spend time with friends”.

The second factor, labeled Life Disruption, summarized a preference of spending time with friends over other obligations. It contained 11 items, including; “I have spent so much time with friends that it has had a negative impact on my job/studies”, “It happens that I spend time with friends instead of sleeping”, and “I have ignored my current/previous partner(s) or family members to spend time with friends”.

The 7-item third factor, labeled Affective Reactions, included the responses where spending time with friends is a response to an emotional state. Items included; “I spend time with friends to reduce feelings of guilt, anxiety, helplessness, & depression”, “I become irritable if I am unable to spend time with friends” and “Lately, I feel I need to spend longer with friends get the same pleasure from hanging out”.

With the remaining Time 1 dataset not previously used for the first EFA (n = 386), we conducted a second EFA and replicated the exploratory structure fit in this independent sample (RMSEA = .068, [.062, .070], TLI = .78, χ2/df = 3.87). We then conducted a confirmatory factor analysis on the complete Time 1 sample, using a diagonally weighted least-squares (DWLS) model suitable for our skewed data, which suggested the three-factor structure did meet the acceptable fit criteria (RMSEA = .080 [.078, .083], TLI = .94, CFI = .94, χ2 = 3666.33, p < .001), as was the case in the independent Time 2 data (RMSEA = .05 [.041, .051], TLI = .98, CFI = .98, χ2 = 981.25, p < .001).

The aggregate response to each domain was retained for analysis and in the complete Time 1 sample. The Social Rumination scores were in the range of MSR = 3.34, SDSR = 0.63, (Min = 1.00, Max = 5.00), Life Disruption scores were in the range of MLD = 2.37, SDLD = 0.65, (Min = 1.00, Max = 4.64), and Affective Reactions scores were in the range of MAR = 2.93, SDAR = 0.81, (Min = 1.00, Max = 5.00).

In the Time 2 dataset, the sample had Social Rumination scores in the range of MSR = 3.38, SDSR = 0.67, (Min = 1.05, Max = 4.75), Life Disruption scores in the range of MLD = 2.34, SDLD = 0.68, (Min = 1.00, Max = 4.36) and Affective Reactions scores in the range of MAR = 2.91, SDAR = 0.83 (Min = 1.00, Max = 4.71).

Test–retest reliability of the O-FAQ

There was a strong, significant, correlation between Time 1 and Time 2 responses to the O-FAQ’s Social Rumination (r(311) = .86, 95% CI [.83, .89], p < .001), Life Disruption (r(311) = .80, 95% CI [.76, .84], p < .001) and Affective Reactions (r(311) = .72, 95% CI [.66, .77], p < .001) scores, demonstrating the test–retest reliability of the factors.

O-FAQ and trait measures

Table 3 reports the correlations between the Big Five traits and the O-FAQ domains. Social Rumination correlated positively to Agreeableness, Neuroticism, and Extraversion, supporting that this factor contains social activity and rumination traits. Similarly, Life Disruption is negatively correlated to Conscientiousness and positively related to Extraversion. Affective Reactions is validated by the positive correlation with Neuroticism and the negative correlation with the planful trait Conscientiousness.

Table 3 Correlations between the Big Five personality traits, DOSPERT domains and full-time 1 O-FAQ factor scores

Table 4 reports the planned two-stage regressions testing first the effect of age, gender, and time with friends and then in a further model with the Big Five traits included. In all models, younger age was associated with higher O-FAQ scores and, apart from Affective Reactions, more time with friends predicted higher scores on the O-FAQ. Gender did not predict domain scores. The inclusion of the Big Five traits improved all models (see Table 4), with the same pattern of significance as in the correlation analysis.

Table 4 Regression models testing age, gender, and time with friends as predictors of the O-FAQ domains (model 1) with a comparison model including the Big Five (model 2) as estimates (with p values in brackets)

O-FAQ and problematic behavior

Presented in Table 3 are the correlations between DOSPERT (risk-taking) behavior and the O-FAQ domains. Social Rumination was positively associated with Recreational, Health, Ethical, and Financial risk-taking. Life Disruption was positively associated with Recreational, Health, Ethical, and Financial risk behaviors, and Affective Reactions was positively associated with Health and Ethical risks. In summary, there is evidence of the O-FAQ being positively related with risky behavior.

O-FAQ and reported time with friends

At Time 1 the responses to the O-FAQ factors significantly positively correlated with the amount of time participants spent with friends in person over the last week; Social Rumination (r(805) = .27, 95% CI [.21, .34], p < .001), Life Disruption (r(805) = .39, 95% CI [.33, .44], p < .001), and Affective Reactions (r(805) = .14, 95% CI [.07, .21], p < .001). The correlation was similar in the Time 2 data; Social Rumination (r(311) = .27, 95% CI [.17, .37], p < .001), Life Disruption (r(311) = .42, 95% CI [.33, .51], p < .001), and Affective Reactions (r(311) = .27, 95% CI [.16, .37], p < .001).

Offline Friend Addiction prevalence

The O-FAQ has 37 items. To be classified as an ‘addict’ using the conservative monothetic criteria, participants needed to provide 37 responses of Neither Agree nor Disagree (3) or higher. For the liberal polythetic criteria, participants would need to provide a response of 3 or higher on 19 items. On average, across the items, participants gave an answer that was coded as 3 or higher on 22/37 items (M > 3 = 21.81, SD > 3 = 7.41, Min = 0, Max = 37).

By the monothetic criteria, we conservatively estimate that, in our full Time 1 sample only five participants (0.62%) were addicted to spending time with their friends. However, in line with similar studies, when using the suggested liberal polythetic classification to form our estimate of the prevalence of offline friend addiction, 558 participants (69%) of the sample were addicts. This polythetic classification is predictable using the amount of time people spend with their friends, (β = .04, z = 6.32, p < .001). Further, the addiction classification can be predicted with low Openness (β = – .43, z = 3.21, p = .001), low Conscientiousness (β = – .47, z = 3.62, p < .001), high Extraversion (β = .62, z = 5.86, p < .001) and high Neuroticism (β = .52, z = 4.88, p < .001).

Discussion

Social media addiction scales often classify people as ‘addicted’ if they engage in higher levels of social information seeking (e.g. Andreassen et al., 2012). As people might also seek social information in offline contexts, we re-worded existing measures of social media 'addiction' to an offline friend addiction context. Using a precedent-defined classification of addiction, we found that 69% of our sample were addicted to their friends, even when offline. On the surface, the prevalence of addicts in this study would present a public health crisis for traditional classes of addiction. However, this is clearly not the case. The remarkably high classification number should make researchers reflect on the literature precedent for classifying ‘pathological’ social behavior.

One would hopefully recognize that conceptually (and indeed pragmatically) this measure is sardonic. ‘Offline friend addiction’ is not an issue of concern for public health, but rather it is capturing the psychology of relatedness seeking and experiences pertinent to social support, adaptation, and well-being across the lifespan (Gauze, Bukowski, Aquan-Assee, & Sippola, 1996; Hartup & Stevens, 1999). Being addicted to spending time with friends is not a recognized psychopathology within academic, clinical, or social domains, nor should it be. We do not advocate the creation of a friend addiction construct. However, it is worth considering how easy it was to classify individuals as pathologically seeking social information using minor adaptations of the instruments used in the context of social networking sites (e.g., Facebook). Interestingly, the original measures have been used as a basis for claims of public health concerns about social networking online (e.g., Andreassen et al., 2012). Our results confirm that researchers can quickly produce farcical results when conceptualizing social media as a distinctive entity to any other social context, akin to the risk of ‘overpathologizing’ everyday life (Billieux, Schimmenti, Khazaal, Maurage, & Heeren, 2015). Seeking information from others about their lives or turning to friends when we feel lonely or bored is to be a social being. However, the wealth of current social media ‘addiction’ research is confounded by failing to demonstrate how these behaviors are unique or divergent from offline social behavior. This also speaks to how behavioral science often considers the impact of technology on everyday life. For example, related work demonstrate that scales that pertain to technology interactions capture sub-facets of mental health rather than actual technology-use behaviors (Davidson, Shaw, & Ellis, 2020).

We are not the first to follow the behavioral addiction literature methodology closely and end with unusual conclusions. There have been concerns raised that existing conceptualizations of behavioral addiction rely on a framework that pathologizes activities that people engage with passionately, by medicalizing repetitive behavior as problematic (Starcevic, Billieux, & Schimmenti, 2018). For example, a recent satirical study measured participants’ ‘addiction’ to Star Wars universe (card) games, as an offline equivalent to Internet Gaming Disorder  (Calvo, Carbonell, Oberst, & Fuster, 2018). They observed that while stated motives to play, such as the desire to explore and socialise, mirrored constructs identified in IGD studies, regular users could not be classified as pathologically ‘addicted’ (Calvo et al., 2018). Calvo et al. called “for caution in diagnosing behavioral addictions; we should refer to other explanatory models such as the passion model or high involvement in an activity” (p. 734). Our findings are in line with these conclusions. We should be notably cautious in the development of novel behavioral addictions in the absence of clear psychiatric criteria or norms. This is particularly important when these behaviors may simply have high scores by their norms. This is the case with social-oriented behavior, where individuals have generally high levels of social interest. While social media addiction is readily pathologized by many researchers, the social processes behind scale questions are social-seeking behaviors, which are prima facie not pathological (and maybe even symptomatic of good well-being) when rephrased to focus on other forms of social behavior. The aim of this study was to sardonically highlight measurement and conceptual issues with developing of novel social media ‘addiction’ scales without due consideration for individuals’ broader social information seeking behavior. We hope that future research in this area will think carefully about their use of scales to capture human behavior which takes place via digital means.

Another explanation for the apparent prevalence of offline friend addicts (as per the current study) and social media addicts (as per previous research) may be the use of polythetic scoring techniques. For example, behavioral addiction diagnoses are often categorically defined on the basis of individuals simply ‘neither agreeing nor disagreeing’ with an arbitrary number of symptoms. This is not a norm-based approach, which is standard in most attempts to identify atypical or pathological behavior. These techniques may present a threshold that is too low to identify ‘unusual’ behavior. Others have noted similar issues with the polythetic-categorical approach (see Cooper, Balsis, & Zimmerman, 2010). Without a norm-based approach to creating cut-off scores, we are diagnosing an addiction based on researchers’ heuristics of how their questionnaires should be responded to. For example, relying on a component model of behavioral addiction (e.g., Griffiths, 2005; Kuss & Griffiths, 2017) to develop scales allows researchers to generate a limitless number of pathologies (including friendship addiction) with almost no theoretical foundation.

Currently, the discourse on issues around measurement and validity in psychology is still growing. Much needed attention has been drawn to ‘questionable measurement practices’ and risks to the internal, external, statistical, and construct validity of new scales (Flake & Fried, 2019). In general, amidst discussions of replication, theory, and confidence crises in psychology (see Oberauer & Lewandowsky, 2019; Pashler & Wagenmakers, 2012), conversations about validity have been under-represented. This is, in part, due to validity being hard to define as a concept (Borsboom, Mellenbergh, & Van Heerden, 2004), but also due to challenges with statistically demonstrating validity. As with the current study (and those it imitates) ‘validation’ is a process of associating novel measures, with their measurement and construct assumptions, with similar extant measures in a problematic tautological ‘jingle-jangle’ fallacy. These attempts at validation in the social media addiction literature could simply be converging on participant response patterns or latent factors that are mis-labeled by different researchers (Davidson et al., 2020). Operationalization of novel psychological constructs remains a challenge for the whole of psychology, but there should be particular caution when developing novel psychopathology measures. We urge caution, patience, and collaboration with informed psychiatric practice before developing novel psychopathologies.

Conclusions

We translated social media addiction measures into an offline friend addiction questionnaire and discovered 69% of our large sample would be classified as addicts. This demonstrates the risks of following common procedures in the social media addiction sub-field when developing new ‘addiction’ scales. To be clear, we do not advocate creating a diagnosis of offline friend addiction. The O-FAQ was created to highlight risks with psychopathologically themed research which lacks meaningful psychiatric validation criteria and uses liberal scoring for classification. Future social media research should focus on testing what components of social media use are distinct to offline social information seeking, especially if attempting to pathologize everyday behavior.