Skip to main content

Neuroplasticity in Humans

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Neuroscience for Psychologists

Abstract

Neuroplasticity describes the ability of human and animal brains to reorganize themselves continuously throughout the lifespan. In addition to the genetic information that is accumulated during evolution to secure the survival of an organism, neuroplasticity serves as a mechanism to cope successfully with ongoing changes of environmental conditions that are not specified by genetic constraints. While prima facie, huge plastic capacities appear desirable, brains require substantial stability to maintain proper functioning. Therefore, neuroplasticity constitutes a trade-off between modifiability and stability.

The present chapter, focused on human neuroplasticity, has two parts: first, rules and basic characteristics of neuroplastic changes are introduced; the second part recapitulates milestones and very recent developments and illustrates the current status of neuroplasticity research in humans. Major factors that modify brains and, by that perception, behavior and cognition, are development and aging as well as changes following brain injuries. Other sources imposing plastic changes come from constraints arising under everyday life conditions. Examples are particularities of occupation including life style and prolonged episodes of heavy schedule of sensory stimulation as exemplarily present in blind Braille readers or musicians. While it takes several 10,000 hours of intense practicing to develop musical skills found in professional musicians, short periods of only a few minutes of practicing can induce neuroplastic changes leading to significant gains in perception and behavior. Accordingly, neuroplasticity is not a unitary process, but numerous mechanisms enable the emergence of different forms of plasticity. The recent development of non-invasive imaging techniques allows you study the impact of neuroplasticity in human brains. This approach confirmed previous findings from animal studies and demonstrated that any possible aspect of brain processing and functioning is altered by plastic reorganization. Large reorganizational changes correlate with large perceptual and behavioral gains and vice versa, indicating that interindividual differences of the outcome of plastic changes have a neural substrate in the amount of plastic capabilities. During the last years, a new and fascinating discipline evolved, where neuroscientists successfully design stimulation conditions based on neuroplasticity mechanisms that do not rely on the conventional modification of use, training, and practicing. By targeting defined brain regions, either through sensory or through direct stimulation of the brain from the outside, plasticity can be induced without any explicit task training. Furthermore, the long-held view that external events are required to drive neuroplasticity has been challenged by observations where the induction of plastic changes is possible by mere mental events without the presence of physical stimuli. From a practical side, understanding the conditions that either facilitate or impede the emergence of neuroplastic changes is a prerequisite for a large scale application of neuroplasticity principles in education as well as in intervention and rehabilitation. However, as neuroplasticity is a rather novel discipline, our current knowledge must necessarily be limited, with many facets of brain changes been overseen due to inappropriate technologies and concepts. Extrapolating from our current knowledge, neuroplasticity will almost certainly offer unimaginable ways of interfering with brains leading to implications unforeseen.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Magnetic source imaging is a combination of Magnetic Resonance Imaging and magnetoencephalography.

  2. 2.

    Gabor patterns are produced by the mathematical Gabor filter used to analyze texture in images. It is believed that neurons or neuronal networks in the visual pathway act as Gabor filters.

References

  • Baltes PB, Lindenberger U (1997) Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: a new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychol Aging 12:12–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelier D, Green CS, Pouget A, Schrater P (2012) Brain plasticity through the life span: learning to learn and action video games. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:391–416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore C, Cooper GF (1970) Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nature 228:477–478

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickwedde M, Krüger MC, Dinse HR (2019) Somatosensory alpha oscillations gate perceptual learning efficiency. Nat Commun 10:263. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-08012-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byl NN, Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM (1996) A primate genesis model of focal dystonia and repetitive strain injury: I. Learning-induced dedifferentiation of the representation of the hand in the primary somatosensory cortex in adult monkeys. Neurology 47:508–520

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Candia V, Wienbruch C, Elbert T, Rockstroh B, Ray W (2003) Effective behavioral treatment of focal hand dystonia in musicians alters somatosensory cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:7942–7946

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapin JK, Moxon KA, Markowitz RS, Nicolelis MA (1999) Real-time control of a robot arm using simultaneously recorded neurons in the motor cortex. Nat Neurosci 2:664–670

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colcombe S, Kramer A, Erickson K, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen N et al (2004) Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:3316–3321

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conde V, Vollmann H, Sehm B, Taubert M, Villringer A, Ragert P (2012) Cortical thickness in primary sensorimotor cortex influences the effectiveness of paired associative stimulation. NeuroImage 60:864–870

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HR, Böhmer G (2002) Plastic-adaptive properties of cortical areas. In: Schüz A, Miller R (eds) Cortical areas: Unity and Diversity: conceptual advances in brain research. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp 311–348

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HR, Merzenich MM (2002) Adaptation of inputs in the somatosensory system. In: Fahle M, Poggio T (eds) Perceptual learning. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 19–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HR, Ragert P, Pleger B, Schwenkreis P, Tegenthoff M (2003) Pharmacological modulation of perceptual learning and associated cortical reorganization. Science 301:91–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HR, Kleibel N, Kalisch T, Ragert P, Wilimzig C, Tegenthoff M (2006) Tactile coactivation resets age-related decline of human tactile discrimination. Ann Neurol 60:88–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HR, Tegenthoff M (2019) Repetitive sensory stimulation – a canonical approach to control the induction of human learning at a behavioural and neural level. In: Manahan-Vaughan D (ed) Handbook of in vivo neural plasticity techniques, A systems neuroscience approach to the neural basis of memory and cognition, vol 28. Academic Press, Cambridge, pp 389–413

    Google Scholar 

  • Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V, Schuierer G, Bogdahn U, May A (2004) Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 427:311–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbert T, Pantev C, Wienbruch C, Rockstroh B, Taub E (1995) Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270:305–307

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaser C, Schlaug G (2003) Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Ann N Y Acad Sci 999:514–517

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson EJ (1953) Improvement in perceptual judgements as a function of controlled practice or training. Psychol Bull 50:401–431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godde B, Berkefeld T, David-Jürgens M, Dinse HR (2002) Age-related changes in primary somatosensory cortex of rats: evidence for parallel degenerative and plastic-adaptive processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:743–752

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone RL (1998) Perceptual learning. Annu Rev Psychol 49:585–612

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heba S, Puts NAJ, Kalisch T, Glaubitz B, Haag LM, Lenz M et al (2016) Local GABA concentration predicts perceptual improvements after repetitive sensory stimulation in humans. Cerebral Cortex 26(3):1295–1301

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb DO (1949) The Organization of Behavior. Wiley and Sons, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillman C, Erickson K, Kramer A (2008) Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:58–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890) Psychology: brief course. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Karim AA, Schneider M, Lotze M, Veit R, Sauseng P, Braun C et al (2010) The truth about lying: inhibition of the anterior prefrontal cortex improves deceptive behavior. Cereb Cortex 20:205–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kattenstroth JC, Kalisch T, Holt S, Tegenthoff M, Dinse HR (2013) Six months of dance intervention enhances postural, sensorimotor, and cognitive performance in elderly without affecting cardio-respiratory functions. Front Aging Neurosci 5:5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2013.00005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleim JA, Chan S, Pringle E, Schallert K, Procaccio V, Jimenez R et al (2006) BDNF val66met polymorphism is associated with modified experience-dependent plasticity in human motor cortex. Nat Neurosci 9:735–737

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krampe RT, Ericsson KA (1996) Maintaining excellence: deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. J Exp Psychol Gen 125:331–359

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kremen WS, Beck A, Elman JA, Gustavson DE, Reynolds CA, Tu XM et al (2019) Influence of young adult cognitive ability and additional education on later-life cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116:2021–2026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lissek S, Wilimzig C, Stude P, Pleger B, Kalisch T, Maier C et al (2009) Immobilization impairs tactile perception and shrinks somatosensory cortical maps. Curr Biol 19:837–842

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RS et al (2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:4398–4403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maniglia M, Seitz AR (2018) Towards a whole brain model of perceptual learning. Curr Opin Behav Sci 20:47–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattson M (2008) Hormesis defined. Ageing Res Rev 7:1–7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich MM, Nelson RJ, Stryker MP, Cynader MS, Schoppmann A, Zook JM (1984) Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. J Comp Neurol 224:591–605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison JH, Hof PR (1997) Life and death of neurons in the aging brain. Science 278:412–419

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nitsche MA, Paulus W (2011) Transcranial direct current stimulation. Restor Neurol Neurosci 29:463–492

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone A, Bartres-Faz D, Keenan JP (1999) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying the brain-behaviour relationship by induction of ’virtual lesions’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 354:1229–1238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pleger B, Foerster AF, Ragert P, Dinse HR, Schwenkreis P, Nicolas V et al (2003) Functional imaging of perceptual learning in human primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. Neuron 40:643–653

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poggio T, Fahle M, Edelman S (1992) Fast perceptual learning in visual hyperacuity. Science 256:1018–1021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pons TP, Garraghty PE, Ommaya AK, Kaas JH, Taub E, Mishkin M (1991) Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferenatation in adult macaques. Science 252:1857–1860

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W (1998) The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture. Brain 121:1603–1630

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Recanzone GH, Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM, Grajski K, Dinse HR (1992) Topographic reorganization of the hand representation in cortical area 3b of owl monkeys trained in a frequency discrimination task. J Neurophysiol 67:1031–1056

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlaug G (2015) Musicians and music making as a model for the study of brain plasticity. Prog Brain Res 217:37–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seitz AR, Dinse HR (2007) A common framework for perceptual learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17:1–6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shibata K, Watanabe T, Sasaki Y, Kawato M (2011) Perceptual learning incepted by decoded fMRI neurofeedback without stimulus presentation. Science 334:1413–1415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW (2003) Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci 6:309–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke E (2008) Effects of music instruction on developing cognitive systems. In: Asbury C, Rich C (eds) Learning, arts, and the brain. Dana Press, New York/Washington, pp 17–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Takesian AE, Hensch TK (2013) Balancing plasticity/stability across brain development. Prog Brain Res 207:3–334

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal P, Miller SL, Bedi G, Byma G, Wang X, Nagarajan SS et al (1996) Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science 271:81–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tartaglia EM, Bamert L, Mast FW, Herzog MH (2009) Human perceptual learning by mental imagery. Curr Biol 19:2081–2085

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe T, Sasaki Y (2015) Perceptual learning: toward a comprehensive theory. Annu Rev Psychol 66:197–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xerri C, Stern JM, Merzenich MM (1994) Alterations of the cortical representation of the rat ventrum induced by nursing behavior. J Neurosci 14:1710–1721

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001) Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 4:759–765

    Google Scholar 

  • Yotsumoto Y, Watanabe T, Sasaki Y (2008) Different dynamics of performance and brain activation in the time course of perceptual learning. Neuron 57:827–833

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hubert R. Dinse .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Dinse, H.R. (2021). Neuroplasticity in Humans. In: Zeise, M.L. (eds) Neuroscience for Psychologists. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-47645-8_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics