Skip to main content

The Art and Science of User Exploitation: AI in the UAE and Beyond

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Artificial Intelligence in the Gulf
  • 569 Accesses

Abstract

The application of artificial intelligence (AI) algorithms is improving everyday tasks worldwide. But while the internet has transformational benefits, it also has its severe drawbacks. Internet infrastructure is extremely expensive and requires large private investment. To profit while giving free access has necessitated the presentation of personalized advertisements. Psychology-based strategies are employed to keep users perpetually engaged, often using emotional or aggressive stimuli that attract attention. Users’ responses and personal data are harvested from multiple sources and analysed through complex statistical algorithms. When hundreds of variables are collected on a person, personality traits, expense patterns, or political beliefs become fairly predictable. This happens because human cognition and emotions evolved for survival in Palaeolithic environments, and certain features are universal. Technology companies sell behaviour prediction models to anyone willing to pay. According to client purposes, users can be prodded to spend money or adopt politically motivated beliefs. Furthermore, smartphone beacons and face recognition technology make it possible to track political activists as well as criminals. Through the use of AI, therefore, tech corporations “design minds” to act as directed and socially engineer societies. Large ethical issues arise, that include privacy concerns, prediction errors, and the empowerment of transnational corporations to profit from directed human activities. As AI becomes part of everyday lives, the internet that intended to bring universal knowledge to the world is unwittingly throwing us back into the Palaeolithic era. Now more than ever, humans ought to become more peaceful and content rather than be driven by ever-increasing emotion-driven contests. This chapter discusses these important issues with the direct or indirect actions that need to be taken to maintain sustainable consumption, world peace, and democratic regimes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Learn more about Nvidia on their: https://www.nvidia.com/en-us/about-nvidia/.

References

  • Abadzi, H. 2020. Memory functions for excellence in the globalisation era. Cambridge Education Reform Reports, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albarracin, D. and Wyer, S. J. 2001. Belief formation, organization, and change: Cognitive and motivational influences. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, and Mark P. Zanna Eds.), Handbook of attitudes and attitude change, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum / 273–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alter, A. 2018. Irresistible: The Rise of Addictive Technology and the Business of Keeping Us Hooked. Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antfolk, J. 2017. Age Limits: Men’s and Women’s Youngest and Oldest Considered and Actual Sex Partners. Evolutionary Psychology January-March 2017: 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704917690401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. M., et al. 2018. Social Psychology, 9th edition. Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, Albert; Ross, D.; Ross, S. 1963. Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66: 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J. 2014. Warriors and worriers. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. 2016. Evolutionary Psychology. The New Science of the Mind. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., and Petty, R. E. 1980), “Sex differences in influenceability: Toward specifying the underlying processes,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 651–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CBS News (2017). Google worker says women don’t advance in tech because of biology. https://www.cbsnews.com/news/google-worker-says-women-dont-advance-in-tech-because-of-biology/.

  • Cialdini, R. 2013. Influence: Science and Practice. 5th edition. New York, Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., and Kallgren, C. A. 1990. A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 6), 1015–1026. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015.

  • Chatzithomas, N., Boutsouki, C., Hatzithomas, L., and Zotos, G.. 2014. Social Media Advertising Platforms: A Cross-cultural Study. International Journal on Strategic Innovative Marketing. Vol. 1(2014) pages. https://doi.org/10.15556/ijsim.01.02.002.

  • Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. 1992. Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J.H.Barkow, L.Cosmides and J.Tooby, The Adapted Mind, chapter 3, pp. 163–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diefenbach, S. and Christoforakos, L. 2017. The Selfie Paradox: Nobody Seems to Like Them Yet Everyone Has Reasons to Take Them. An Exploration of Psychological Functions of Selfies in Self-Presentation, Frontiers in Psychology 2017. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00007.

  • Durante, K. M. and Griskevicius, V. 2016. Evolution and consumer behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 27–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.025.

  • Etgar, S. and Amichai-Hamburger, Y. 2017. Not All Selfies Took Alike: Distinct Selfie Motivations Are Related to Different Personality Characteristics. Frontiers in Psychology, 26,(8)‚ 842. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00842.

  • Festinger, L. and Carlsmith, J. M. 1959. Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58(2), 203. Retrieved fromhttp://web.mit.edu/curhan/www/docs/Articles/15341_Readings/Motivation/Festinger_Carlsmith_1959_Cognitive_consequences_of_forced_compliance.pdf.

  • Festinger, L. and Maccoby, N. 1964. On resistance to persuasive communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogg, B. J. 2002. Persuasive Technology: Using Computers to Change What We Think and Do Interactive Technologies. Morgan-Kaufman Series in Interactive Technologies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J and Koelling, R. A. 1966. Relation of Cue to Consequence in Avoidance Learning. Psychonomic science 41):123–124. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03342209.

  • Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R.W., and Malone, P. S. 1993. You can’t not believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, volume 65, issue 2, pp. 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. 1964. “The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I”. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 7 1): 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-519364)90038-4.

  • Hamzany, Y., Feinmesser, R., Shpitzer, T., et al. 2013. Is Human Saliva an Indicator of the Adverse Health Effects of Using Mobile Phones? Antioxidants & Redox Signaling, Vol 18, No. 6, p. 622.  https://doi.org/10.1089/ars.2012.4751.

  • Hare, R. M. and Simmons, L. W. 2019. Sexual selection and its evolutionary consequences in female animals. Biological Reviews, 94, 929–956. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12484.

  • Harrari, Y. N. 2018. 21 Lessons for the 21st Century. New York: Spiegel & Grau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins S.T., Jones, C. and Rholes, W. 1977. Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 132:141–154· https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-103177)80007-3

  • Hovland, C. I. and Weiss, W. 1951. The Influence of Source Credibility on Communication Effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly. Vo. 15, No. 4, pp. 635–650, https://doi.org/10.1086/266350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ilkka, T. 2018. The Impact of Artificial Intelligence on Learning, Teaching, and Education. European Union. Publications Office of the European Union.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. 2011. Thinking Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J. and Chance, S. 2002. Decisions and the Evolution of Memory: Multiple Systems, Multiple Functions. Psychological Review, Vol. 109, No. 2, 306–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein-Flugge, M. C., Kennerley, S. W., Friston, K., and Bestmann, S. 2016. Neural Signatures of Value Comparison in Human Cingulate Cortex during Decisions Requiring an Effort-Reward Trade-off. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 39: 10002. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0292-16.2016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kool, V. K. and Agrawal, R. 2016. Psychology of Technology. New York, Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A.and Alwin, D. F. 1989. Aging and susceptibility to attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 416–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, J. J., Gino, F., Jin, E. S., Rice L., and Josephs, R. A. 2015. Hormones and ethics: Understanding the Biological basis of unethical conduct. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol. 144, No. 5, pp. 891–897. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000099.

  • Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., and Karpinski, A. C. 2014. The relationship between cell phone use, academic performance, anxiety, and Satisfaction with Life in college students. Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 31, p. 343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.

  • Liberini, F., Redoano, M., Russo, A. et al. 2018. Politics in the Facebook Era Evidence from the 2016 US Presidential Elections. University of Warwick, Report no. No. 389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, C. C., and Anderson, C. 2015. The downside of looking like a leader: Power, nonverbal confidence, and participative decision-making. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 42–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.12.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E., Miller, D.G. and Burns, H.J. 1978. Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, Vol. 4, pp. 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19.

  • Lopez-Fernandez. O. 2015. Cross-Cultural Research on Internet Addiction: A Systematic Review. International Archives of Addiction Research and Medicine, Volume 1, Issue 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machin A J, and Fisher D 2015. Why does engaging with families in maternal and infant care make such a positive difference? International Journal for Birth and Parenting Education 3: 19–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machin, A. 2017. Humans cannot deal with the internet phenomena because they never evolved to cope with them.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGonigal, J. 2011. Reality is Broken: Why Games Make us Better and How they Can Change the World. New York, NY, US: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, R., Kishida, K., Lohrenz, T. et al. 2018. The Protective Action Encoding of Serotonin Transients in the Human Brain, Neuropsychopharmacology, 43. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2017.304.

  • Nagle J. E., Brodsky, S. L. and Weeter, K. 2014. Gender, Smiling, and Witness Credibility in Actual Trials. Behavioral Sciences and the Law‚ 32(2), 195–206. https://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.2112.

  • Nightingale, S. J., Wade, K. A., and Watson, D. G. 2017. Can people identify original and manipulated photos of real-world scenes? Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, Vol. 2, No. 30. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-017-0067-2.

  • Nordgren, L. F., Bos, M. W. and Dijksterhuis, A. 2011. The best of both worlds: Integrating conscious and unconscious thought best solves complex decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 509–511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.007.

  • Pearson, J. C. 1982. The Role of Gender in Source Credibility. Note: Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication Association 68th, Louisville, KY, November 4–7, 1982. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED226390.

  • Piha, S. 2018. Evolutionary Psychology for Consumers: Awareness of ultimate explanations as a self-reflective tool for consumer empowerment. Economics Department, University of Turku.

    Google Scholar 

  • Previc, F. 2011. The Dopaminergic Mind in Human Evolution and History. Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, M. E., Sheehy-Skeffington, J., Sidnaius, J., and Pound, N. 2017. Is sociopolitical egalitarianism related to bodily and facial formidability in men? Evolution and Human Behavior, 385), 626–634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.04.001.

  • Purves, D., LaBar, K. S., Platt, M. L. Et al. 2012. Principles of Cognitive Neuroscience. Sinauer Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi and V. Gallese. 2002. “From mirror neurons to imitation: Facts and speculations.” In The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases, edited by A.N. Meltzoff and W. Prinz. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 247–266.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Savitsky, K., V.H. Medvec, and T. Gilovich. 1997. “Remembering and Regretting: The Zeigarnik Effect and the Cognitive Availability of Regrettable Actions and Inactions.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23: 248–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schradie, J. 2019. The Revolution That Wasn’t: How Digital Activism Favors Conservatives. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, D. O. (1981). Life stage effects on attitude change, especially among the elderly. In S. B. Kiesler, J. N. Morgan, & V. K. Oppenheimer (Eds.), Aging: Social change (pp. 183–204). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E., and Hager, J. L. 1972. Biological boundaries of learning. East Norwalk, CT, US: Appleton-Century-Croft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. and Fernbach, P. 2017. The Knowledge Illusion: Why We Never Think Alone. Riverhead Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgras, J. G., Dengah, H., J., Lacy, M. G. et al. 2018. Social genomics of healthy and disordered internet gaming, American Journal of Human Biology. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23146.

  • Spanovic, M., Lickel, B. and Denson, T. F. 2010. Fear and anger as predictors of motivation for intergroup aggression: Evidence from Serbia and Republika Srpska. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, Vol 13, No. 6, pp. 725–739. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430210374483.

  • Supekar, K., Kochalka, J. Schaer, M. et al. 2018. Deficits in mesolimbic reward pathway underlie social interaction impairments in children with autism. Brain. 2018 Sep 1;1419): 2795–2805. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy191.

  • Stanton, Steven J. 2017. The role of testosterone and estrogen in consumer behavior and social and economic decision making: A review. Hormones and Behavior, 92: 155–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.11.006. Epub 2016 Nov 11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ten Brinke, L., Vohs, K. D., Carney, D. R. 2016. Can Ordinary People Detect Deception After All? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, August 2016, Vol. 20, No. 8 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.012.

  • Todd P. M., Hertwig R., and Hoffrage U. 2005. Evolutionary cognitive psychology. The handbook of evolutionary psychology pp. 776–802. Wiley, 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030-5774.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. 1996. Friendship and the Banker’s Paradox: Other pathways to the Evolution of Adaptations for Altruism. Proceedings of the British Academy. Retrieved August 26, 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  • TRA. 2019. The UAE National Cyber Strategy. Retrieved from https://government.ae/en/about-the-uae/strategies-initiatives-and-awards/federal-governments-strategies-and-plans/national-cybersecurity-strategy-2019.

  • Trivers, R. L. 1971. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 461, 35–57. Retrieved August 25, 2018, from greatergood.berkeley.edu/images/uploads/Trivers-EvolutionReciprocalAltruism.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tufekci, Z. 2018. YouTube, the Great Radicalizer. New York Times, March 10, 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  • UNESCO. 2017. School Violence and Bullying: Global Status Report.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, P. 2016. The Psychology of the Internet (2nd ed). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. and Peterson, R. 1996. Demonic Males. Apes and the Origins of Human Violence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • YouGov. 2018. Childhood Obesity May be Linked to Excessive Use of Technology. Retrieved from http://www.mena.yougov.com/en/news/2018/07/16/childhood-obesity-may-be-linked-excessive-use-tech/.

  • Zimmermann, K. A., and Emspak, J. 2017, June 27. Internet History Timeline: ARPANET to the World Wide Web [Web log post]. Retrieved July 16, 2018, from https://www.livescience.com/20727-internet-history.html.

  • Zeigarnik, B.V. 1969. Introduction to Pathopsychology. Oxford, England: Moscow University.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendix – Evolutionary Psychology: Essential Concepts

Appendix – Evolutionary Psychology: Essential Concepts

This branch of psychology examines mental functions from the perspective of human and animal survival. All organisms since the earth was formed survived and reproduced by adapting to the conditions around them. This is called natural selection. Those that could not leave viable offspring became extinct. Homo sapiens ultimately descends from single-cell organisms and a long line of intermediate animals. For survival, physical functions, as well as adapted behaviours, were necessary.

The behaviours of organisms evolved to adapt to the environments where they lived. Thus memory, motivation, social interactions, or personality features can be examined from the perspective of their utility for survival in humans and non-human animals (Tooby and Cosmides 1996; Buss 2016). The aim is to understand the processes that designed the human mind in order to resolve issues that are not easily explainable as independent phenomena (Piha 2018). Hypotheses informed by evolution can be tested (Durante and Griskevicius 2016).

Many behavioural adaptations probably developed in the Palaeolithic era, when humans were hunters and gatherers (about 2.5 million to 10,000 years ago; also called sometimes “stone age”). They experienced include nomadic, kin-based lifestyle in small groups, long life and low fertility for mammals, long female pregnancy and lactation, cooperative hunting and aggression, tool use, and the sexual division of labour. They also dealt with predators and prey, food acquisition and sharing, mate choice, child-rearing, interpersonal aggression, interpersonal assistance, diseases and a host of other fairly predictable challenges that constituted significant selection pressures. Successful behavioural adaptations arose over millennia of pressures, for example, to hunt large and dangerous animals.

The individuals carrying the most successful adaptations reproduced and left offspring that were more likely to display the traits that favoured survival. The advent of sexual reproduction facilitated the exchange of genes for survival in more challenging environments, so sexual selection has been a powerful selection force. The organisms able to mate with individuals possessing traits that favoured survival could have offspring that survived in subsequent generations. Genes carry DNA codes that influence various traits, so evolutionary psychology takes the “gene’s eye view”. If you were a gene what would you do to survive? In some respects, genes are agnostic. There seems to be no forward-thinking or general plan for better-adapted animals. The only criterion that matters is offspring survival to adulthood and reproduction.

Natural and sexual selection are not the only ways genes change. Chance variations over time (called genetic drift) also produce genetic expressions that may be harmful, harmless, or neutral. Some traits are merely “carried along” by an adaptive trait. Some are merely consequences of behaviours, such as the existence of belly buttons. Also, a trait can evolve because it served one particular function, but subsequently, it may come to serve another (exaptation). One example would be reading in humans, which uses circuits specialized for face recognition and the detection of small objects. It is often difficult to determine which behaviours are a direct outcome of evolutionary forces and which ones are carried along.

Survival strategies are manifest in all organisms. Even relatively simple organisms have the means to sense the environment and take action, avoiding harmful events and maximizing feeding. Nervous systems and hormonal molecules have generated mechanisms such as sensory and motor systems, memory and emotions, and motivation. And many animals found it advantageous to live in groups, such as bacterial colonies. The biological basis of behaviours is not always obvious, but existing research confirms many biological links.

Nevertheless, behaviours are not “hardwired” or genetically determined in a straightforward way. Multiple genes, as well as epigenetic mechanisms, typically affect behaviours. Humans are born with nervous systems that are malleable, and they become adapted to the environments they live in. And in various times of history, humans have encountered different cultures and technologies and adapted to those. Cultures also depend on human capabilities, so the possibilities are not infinite. Our behavioural systems today appear to function in ways that are quite different from those of 3000 years ago, but genes change slowly. People can only use capabilities whose essential features exist. For example, humans do not automatically use brainwaves to control equipment, though many people seem capable of learning this skill. And it is unlikely that the population can be trained to move large items from a distance, fly, or see behind walls.

In brief, the following are some evolutionary psychology concepts that seem relevant to internet phenomena.

Inclusive fitness is a concept which implies that people may support the offspring of relatives who carry their genes (Hamilton 1964). To avoid incest and expect support, humans (and many animals) privilege their closest relatives and give them much care (Trivers 1971). Evocations of brotherhood, kindred, lineage, or close communal links may increase care and support. One example is the many internet searches for long-lost relatives, genealogy, surnames, and genetic testing.

Unlike most animals, human males care for their offspring and defend them. To maximize their reproductive chances, they use complex long- and short-term mating strategies (Buss 2016). The typical female oestrus cycle of the animals was replaced by a hidden menstrual cycle, making it hard for men to detect fertility. Complex mating strategies ensued from this phenomenon, including a demand for fidelity, female coyness, and mutual attempts at deception.

One important strategy concerns the features of women who are to raise men’s children. Men are primarily interested in young women, whose reproductive life is long (Artfolk 2017). The appearance of youth, therefore, has become a primary concern of women. Selfies attest to an interest in looking thin, to the point of digital manipulation. The desire for youth may be one reason for the emergence of child pornography.

Men must entice the most suitable women with resources suggesting the ability to support the young. They must also be considered worthy comrades. Important to male support strategy is the acquisition of resources and power that they then use to signal competence to women who need childbearing support and to attract male followers. One consequence is a tendency for males to be selfish and acquire power. Internet entrepreneurs and leaders have been almost exclusively male and have, among others, focused on power acquisition through users’ data.

Humans have a mild degree of polygyny; women are about 18% smaller than men, and this dimorphism helps men prevail violently when opportunities arise. Women have had to adapt to abuse, while at the same time seeking strong males who will protect their families. Polygyny means that women must fight other women over men’s resources. This puzzling phenomenon seems to be reflected in workplaces and in social media, where women tend to forgive straying men and attack other women.

Perhaps more than other mammals, humans have survived in communities. Groups were small, about 40–150 individuals, judging from contemporary hunter-gatherers. Arguably, people are unprepared to process the features of 1500 Facebook “friends”.

Hunter-gatherer groups had a division of labour where women were occupied with child-rearing and gathering plants. Men hunted in male-only groups for animals that provided protein. Therefore, men have a bias towards single-gender groups.

Communal living in a precarious era seems to have given rise to multiple cognitive rules. To survive in groups of people who are not close relatives, humans follow a social contract among their members, delineating benefits and obligations on the basis of reciprocal altruism. People who coexist in communities are bound by social contracts, that is rules of cooperation for mutual benefit (Cosmides and Tooby 1992).

The collaboration between individuals tries following a tit-for-tat rule. Members must contribute as well as benefit. To ensure wise management of scarce resources, humans (and some primates) have developed abilities to spot and punish free-riders and cheats. In fact, a morality system ingrained in humans has developed for this purpose. The punishment or detection of deviants is regulated by social contracts. Revenge against violators causes pleasure. When social contracts do not exist, the punishment may be excessive (Cosmides and Tooby 1992).

Status is a powerful social force among animals and also humans. Skilful Palaeolithic hunters, stronger individuals, and those protected by more aggressive people left more offspring (Buss 2016). In principle, its role should be reduced in the modern era, but from a very early age, children seem attuned to status displays around them (Benenson 2014). Status seems to be established relatively quickly in schools and also in online groups. Men and women vie for status given their different roles, and marketers of various types take advantage of this desire. In an online environment, personal impressions are missing, but confident written expressions may serve to establish expertise, even when it does not exist (Locke and Anderson 2015).

Bullying seems to constitute efforts to increase status. It implies the use of superior strength and perception of greater benefits from a status increase. Higher status seems linked to credibility; that is, people may believe those who display indicators of high statuses, such as self-confidence and a number of allies. Women are often bullied online by both sexes, and much distress results from such events, particularly among adolescents.

Humans are also only one of two species that attack individuals of the same species. Thus, men have been embroiled in wars in just about every generation (Wrangham and Peterson 1996). Men seem specialized for fighting with their hands (Hare and Simmons 2019). One prominent feature of male attacks is the abduction of women, who will bear and raise the next generation. This feature has profound implications for sexual selection. Men fight with each other for the right to mate with women. Women vie and compete with other women for the attention of strong men who can protect and feed their children. One problem with this strategy is an increasing level of violence with subsequent generations, as women bear “sexy sons”.

One consequence of male alliances towards hunting or abducting women is a rejection of women in their groups. They are less fit for battle, and they are certainly not interested in abducting more women. The male tendency for single-gender groups has been the exclusion of women from technological positions, as well as elaborate explanations about women being unfit for technology (as presented by a Google employee in 2017) (CBS News 2017). Sexual assaults and harassment may also be expressions of this ancestral feature.

Male fighting has had multiple effects in history. Men rely on others to follow orders, so they seem able to abide by strict hierarchies. Wars and conflict engender efforts to overcome the opponent, thus creating technological progress. Men compete among themselves and tend to be prejudiced towards men of other groups. But they also create alliances, a concept that is called “cooperative competitiveness” (Buss 2016; Benenson 2014). Men’s ability to form alliances towards a greater goal is evident in the development of hardware and software, as well as in the advertisement alliances that collect and apply people’s data.

Evidence for some of these traits comes from young children. Young boys show the signs of aggression and war-related play that parents may have never taught them (Benenson 2014). This may be one reason for the tendency towards motor expertise through videogames and the desire for multi-player videogames. Research also shows that strong men are less interested in equity (Price et al. 2017). Thus, groups of young males may espouse totalitarian political beliefs, and they may have increased aggressive displays as a result of violent media exposure.

The strategy for winning wars often leads to the use of deceptive tactics. Both genders use deception, but men are more likely than women to engage in various practices, and testosterone levels seem related to this propensity (Lee et al. 2015; Stanton 2017). The incidence of theft and deception that has prevailed on the internet that was to be open and egalitarian suggests trends that are not random. In effect, hacking is modern warfare. The strategies combine technical and psychological behaviours. They may hark to the role of testosterone, which increases the opportunity to trick people. The players on both sides find it stimulating, and two sides are needed for cooperative competitiveness. Evidence suggests that the male-only groups that created and promoted the internet treated its many challenges in terms of war-like offence and defence. Much of the terminology, such as “killer apps”, attests to the male propensity for aggression.

Communal living and the decision mechanisms about it have big implications individuals inhabiting the internet. Like many animals, humans do not think alone about complex issues. Knowledge is distributed in a community, and it may be transmitted within a group through majority-biased learning (Sloman and Fernbach 2017). In fact, education is one strategy to disseminate community-based knowledge in an organized manner. One implication of communal knowledge is a tendency to believe what the majority says rather than doubt it. As mentioned above, our decisions are influenced by the number and status of people who have various opinions. Our brain collects statistics about these events. For people to understand a statement, they must have an initial belief about it. Understanding is believing (Gilbert et al. 1993). People may read or hear something, and then refute it through critical thinking. This feature creates a bias in favour of stated beliefs and gives an advantage to advertisers. Corporations create social illusions to manipulate the digital currency, which signals value to users. One means is fake accounts and images created to give the impression that many people support a certain position.

Social media heavily invest in the concepts of social contracts and reciprocal altruism (Trivers 1971; altruism consists of helping others at a cost to ourselves). Companies goad users to reply to messages and ensure that responses by default are open, enabling others to exert social control over this essential moral function. A user who refuses could be labelled a violator. But this requires a personal investment. By responding to comments, sending “liking” comments; one becomes a slave to the system and spends many hours creating content for advertising companies. People may feel good; certain brain areas are linked to social exchanges and linkage to the limbic system results in dopamine release (Purves et al. 2012). However, many interactions are generated by computers. Humans have had no relevant evolutionary pressures in distinguishing humans from software messages.

One important feature and community-based manipulation involves the “fear of missing out”. People seem unwilling to be left alone, perhaps as an adaptation from millennia of community survival. Social media show friends engaged in interesting activities or purchases and websites emphasize that users must participate in order to keep up. Conformism is another important feature used, particularly when information is limited. Women seem particularly prone to conformity (Pearson 1982; Nagle et al. 2014). And the current status of the internet, with written messages coming from unknown parties, seems to be a fruitful environment for this phenomenon. Marketing and news websites show the numbers of people who share or respond to certain messages, though the veracity of the numbers is uncertain. Social herding is expected, that is users are to follow the hints of others in opinions or in marketing.

Scarcity was a reality in the era before abundant food and basic infrastructure. Animals are set up to eat however much they can when they find food. The threat of scarcity, therefore, seems to be a powerful carry over into the modern era. Corporations market merchandise by evoking scarcity; “buy while supplies last”, buy today. Evidence points towards a neural network that governs social interactions, and it’s heavily linked to the mesolimbic dopamine pathway and therefore excitement and emotional centres (Supekar et al. 2018). Therefore, positive and negative interactions are rewarding. People find opportunities to improve status by humiliating others, such as taking compromising pictures and posting them online. These variables are not conscious, so people cannot easily say what they are doing and why. It may just seem “fun”.

Humans learn a great deal from their companions. Children are able to imitate extensively, and there is a dedicated neurological circuitry for observational learning (e.g. mirror neurons; Rizzolatti et al. 2002). This suggests that they are likely to imitate high-status people similar to them. Aggressive and emotional expressions are sometimes beyond people’s control, particularly when they have been trained from childhood by violent media.

One of the many applications for this pertains to “internet influencers”, people who have large numbers of followers. Also, YouTube, owned by Google, is a prime medium for performance-related displays and seems to have become an important conservative recruitment strategy. As with all advertisers, viewers are encouraged to keep watching as long as possible, and people are drawn to content that is more extreme than their original position. So a business practice may be responsible for increasing the worldwide incidence of extreme views (Tufekci 2018).

The challenges among groups of males result in technological progress. Little by little over the millennia, humans have overcome the ancestral living conditions. We now live in warm housing, we are safe from predators, we have organized education, and have optimized food sources. Thus, the inherited aspects of our mental apparatus interact with the environment in new ways that are only partly explored. Clearly, humans have not been subjected to aeons of natural selection for many features that are now common in our lives (Machin and Fisher 2015).

What helped survival and reproduction million years ago does not make for suitable behaviour in the twenty-first century (Harrari 2018; Kool and Agrawal 2016). People over the millennia interacted with others in person, rather than, say, as disembodied voices. They can detect deception in face-to-face interactions (ten Brinke et al. 2016), but not online. Also, the ancestral environment did not include pressures to distinguish between real and fake people. Accordingly, people cannot identify fake photos reliably (Nightingale et al. 2017). In person, people are often able to detect lying or duplicity, but they tend to believe statements seen in writing. Furthermore, people tend to conform when they are unsure of their role (Cialdini et al. 1990). Thus, they may become liable to suggestions. When a piece of software asks to fill out surveys or accept an invitation, it is difficult to conceive that there is no human behind the screen. Genetic linkages of traits in conjunction with new environments open a raft of possibilities that have not been encountered before. Clearly, the biological roots of behaviour are only partly understood, and new explanations are not always easy. Furthermore, humans display a lot of variability in their traits. But the uniformity of some behaviours like taking selfies (Diefenbach et al. 2017) shows the utility of exploring evolution in hopes of improving quality of life under these new circumstances.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Abadzi, H., ElAsad, S. (2021). The Art and Science of User Exploitation: AI in the UAE and Beyond. In: Azar, E., Haddad, A.N. (eds) Artificial Intelligence in the Gulf. Palgrave Macmillan, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-16-0771-4_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-16-0771-4_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-16-0770-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-16-0771-4

  • eBook Packages: Economics and FinanceEconomics and Finance (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics