Skip to main content

Evolution of Intelligence, The

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Definition

Definitions of intelligence in the scientific literature center around the idea that that intelligence refers to cognitive processes that allow humans and other animals to solve novel problems in their environments. Thus, Jerison (1973, pp. 16–17) notes that “Although experts differ with regard to the nature of human intelligence, a more or less common ground is that it is a dimension of cognitive behavior – the way one knows the world and the way one uses that knowledge when adapting to changing conditions.”

Introduction

However, intelligence is not the only set of mechanisms designed to enable organisms to cope with environmental novelty. Intelligence is usually distinguished from learning which subsumes a variety of mechanisms that allow the organism to take advantage of temporary regularities in its environment – paradigmatically classical and operant conditioning (MacDonald 2013). Social learning is also usually distinguished from intelligence. Unlike classical and...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alexander, R. D. (1989). Evolution of the human psyche. In P. Mellars & C. Stringer (Eds.), The human revolution: Behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans (pp. 455–513). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carew, T. J., Walters, E. T., & Kandel, E. R. (1981). Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica. The Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 1426–1437.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, P., Just, M., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Demetriou, A., Platsidou, M., & Kazi, S. (2001). Integrating concepts and tests of intelligence from the differential and developmental traditions. Intelligence, 29, 307–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, D., & MacDonald, K. B. (2005). The evolution of domain-general mechanisms in intelligence and learning. Journal of General Psychology, 132, 5–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2002). Unraveling the enigma of human intelligence: Evolutionary psychology and the multimodular mind. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence (pp. 145–198). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crinella, F. M., & Yu, J. (1995). Brain mechanisms in problem solving and intelligence: A replication and extension. Intelligence, 21, 225–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20, 204–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandes, H. B. F., Woodley, M. A., & te Nijenhuis, J. (2014). Differences in cognitive abilities among primates are concentrated on G: Phenotypic and phylogenetic comparisons with two meta-analytical databases. Intelligence, 46, 311–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galsworthy, M. J., Arden, R., & Chabris, C. F. (2014). Animal models of general cognitive ability for genetic research into cognitive functioning. In D. Finkel & C. A. Reynolds (Eds.), Behavior genetics of cognition across the lifespan (pp. 257–278). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2015). Social competition and the evolution of fluid intelligence. In S. Goldstein, D. Princiotta, & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of intelligence: Evolutionary theory, historical perspective, and current concepts (pp. 105–119). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heinrich, B. (2000). Testing insight in ravens. In C. Heyes & L. Huber (Eds.), The evolution of cognition (pp. 289–305). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jerison, H. J. (1973). The evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., & Engle, R. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larson, G., & Saccuzzo, D. (1989). Cognitive correlates of general intelligence: Toward a process theory of g. Intelligence, 13, 5–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E., & Finkelstein, A. (1997). Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behaviour, 53, 549–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R. (2006). Race differences in intelligence: An evolutionary analysis. Augusta: Washington Summit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, K. B. (2008). Effortful control, explicit processing and the regulation of human evolved predispositions. Psychological Review, 115, 1012–1031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, K. B. (2013). Human general intelligence as a domain general psychological adaptation. In J. Kush (Ed.), Intelligence quotient: Testing, role of genetics and the environment and social outcomes (pp. 35–54). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potts, R. (1998). Variability selection in hominid evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 7, 81–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 99, 4436–4441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1017–1027.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, J. P. (2004). Placing intelligence into an evolutionary framework, or how g fits into the r-K matrix of life history traits including longevity. Intelligence, 32, 321–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, J. P., & Jensen, A. R. (2010). The rise and fall of the Flynn effect as a reason to expect the narrowing of the Black–White gap. Intelligence, 38, 213–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H., Parker, T., Wilkinson, A., Hegion, A., & Fish, E. (1976). Longitudinal study of individual differences in cognitive development and scholastic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 377–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, A. H., Elliffe, D. M., Hunt, G. R., Emery, N. J., Clayton, N. S., & Gray, R. D. (2011). New caledonian crows learn the functional properties of novel tool types. PLoS ONE, 6(12), e26887.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vrba, E. S. (1995). The fossil record of African antelopes (Mammalia, Bovidae) in relation to human evolution and paleoclimate. In E. S. Vrba, G. H. Denton, T. C. Partridge, & L. H. Burckle (Eds.), Paleoclimate and evolution, with emphasis on human origins (pp. 385–424). New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodley of Menie, M. A., Fernandes, H. B. F., & Hopkins, W. D. (2015). The more g-loaded, the more heritable, evolvable, and phenotypically variable: Homology with humans in chimpanzee cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 50, 159–163.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kevin MacDonald .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

MacDonald, K., Woodley of Menie, M.A. (2021). Evolution of Intelligence, The. In: Shackelford, T.K., Weekes-Shackelford, V.A. (eds) Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-19650-3_3093

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics