Skip to main content
Log in

The Roots of Racial Categorization

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

I examine the origins of ordinary racial thinking. In doing so, I argue against the thesis that it is the byproduct of a unique module (e.g. a folk-biology module). Instead, I defend a pluralistic thesis according to which different forms of racial thinking are driven by distinct mechanisms, each with their own etiology. I begin with the belief that visible features are diagnostic of race. I argue that the mechanisms responsible for face recognition have an important, albeit delimited, role to play in sustaining this belief. I then argue that essentialist beliefs about race are driven by some of the mechanisms responsible for “entitativity perception”: the tendency to perceive some aggregates of people as more genuine groups than others. Finally, I argue that coalitional thinking about race is driven by a distinctive form of entitativity perception. However, I suggest that more data is needed to determine the prevalence of this form of racial thinking.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For example, see https://home.chicagopolice.org/about/contact-us/how-to-describe-a-suspect/

  2. Kramer and colleagues’ (2017) use of the term “accurately” needs to be qualified, for if racial categories are not real, there is nothing for the model to be accurate about. Rather than presupposing realism, we can interpret Kramer and colleagues’ statement as follows: the model’s categorization of an individual as Black was “accurate” only if that individual would be considered Black by the folk; and the model’s categorization of an individual as White was “accurate” only if that individual would be considered White by the folk. This interpretation fits with Kramer and colleagues’ methodology, for the faces they fed into the model were found on Google Images, with search terms, such as “Black women,” and “White men.”

  3. Below, in section 2.3, I defend a somewhat weaker claim.

  4. I have been focusing on face recognition, but it is worth pointing out that in recognizing others, people utilize non-facial bodily cues as well (e.g., see Rice et al. 2013). This is compatible with the recognition-byproduct hypothesis.

  5. According to convention, the modern era began with the arrival of Columbus in “The New World,” in 1492. Hochman (2019) argues that the concept of race actually emerged in late medieval Spain, just before the start of the modern era. Obviously, this is still well after our capacity for face recognition first emerged.

  6. In what follows, I use the term “essentialism” to denote broad essentialism (as defined above in section 3.3).

  7. No doubt, various other factors have a role to play in explaining this sort of case. For example, knowing that Italian-Americans immigrated from the same geographical region may have caused some to construe them as sharing an inherited essence. Moreover, there is evidence that people with a “social dominance orientation”—i.e. a general preference for social inequality—tend to biologize and dehumanize social outgroups (Costello and Hodson 2010).

  8. The account I have just defended is compatible with the view that racial categorization is a modern phenomenon. Plausibly, before the modern concept of race emerged, the most salient social groups cut across racial lines. For instance, Ivan Hannaford argues persuasively that before the Renaissance and the Reformation, “one’s identity and social status were determined by one’s political and religious commitments, not the color of one’s skin” (Hannaford 1996, pp. 147−148). Once racial markers became salient cues for group membership, this would have engaged the mechanisms responsible for categorical entitativity perception, thereby fueling the acquisition of distinctively modern, essentialist conceptions, of race. What might have caused racial markers to become salient, though? A large body of research has found that people tend to form social networks with those who are similar to them. Psychologists refer to this as homophily (for an overview, see McPherson et al. 2001). There has been little research into how people weigh the different dimensions of similarity (e.g. race versus age) when forming social networks. Plausibly, if one’s social standing is largely determined by one’s religious and political beliefs, the visible markers of race will not be especially salient. However, in a society in which these beliefs are less powerful drivers of social standing, racial markers may be salient enough to shape the emergence of homophilous social networks. The emergence of these racially skewed networks will, in turn, increase the salience of racial markers. Of course, there is a lot more to be said about the historical and psychological factors that drive racial segregation. Suffice it to say that additional research into the multidimensional nature of homophily may provide important insights.

  9. See Wilkerson (2020) for a discussion of the resemblances between social castes and racial groups.

  10. For helpful discussion and feedback, I would like to thank Edouard Machery, Ron Mallon, Adam Hochman, Joshua Glasgow, and members of the WashU Mind and Perception Group. I would also like to thank two reviewers for their extensive and insightful feedback.

References

  • Astuti, R., G. E. A. Solomon, and S. Carey. 2004. Constraints on cognitive development: A case study of the acquisition of folkbiological and folksociological knowledge in Madagascar. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development  69 (3): i, v, vii–viii, 1–161.

  • Atran, S. 1990. Cognitive foundations of natural history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S. 2001. Comment on F. J. Gil-White’s article ‘Are ethnic groups biological “species” to the human brain? Essentialism in our cognition of some social categories. Current Anthropology 42 (4): 537–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S., and D. Medin. 2010. The native mind and the cultural construction of nature. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balas, B. 2012. Bayesian face recognition and perceptual narrowing in face-space. Developmental Science 15 (4): 579–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balas, B., J. Peissig, and M. Moulson. 2015. Children (but not adults) judge similarity in own- and other-race faces by the color of their skin. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 130: 56–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baudouin, J., and M. Gallay. 2006. Is face distinctiveness gender based? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32: 789–798.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. 2014. Seeing-as in the light of vision science. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 89 (1): 560–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P., and C. Veres. 1999. The perceived intentionality of groups. Cognition 71 (1): B1–B9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chas, A., V. Betancor, N. Delgado, and A. Rodríguez-Pérez. 2018. Children consider their own group to be more human than other social groups: Evidence from indirect and direct measures. Social Psychology 49 (3): 125–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condit, C.M., R.L. Parrott, T.M. Harris, J. Lynch, and T. Dubriwny. 2004. The role of “genetics” in popular understandings of race in the United States. Public Understanding of Science 13: 249–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., J. Tooby, and R. Kurzban. 2003. Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive Science 7 (4): 173–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, K., and G. Hodson. 2010. Exploring the roots of dehumanization: The role of animal-human similarity in promoting immigrant humanization. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 13: 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, M.T., S.J. Sherman, and D.L. Hamilton. 2002. Perceived entitativity, stereotype formation, and the interchangeability of group members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83: 1076–1094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, C.D., M.J. Rasch, I. Bülthoff, and C. Chen. 2016. Integration or separation in the processing of facial properties—A computational view. Scientific Reports 6: 20247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., M.R. Banaji, and R.P. Abelson. 1999. Group entitativity and group perception: Associations between physical features and psychological judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 991–1003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deeb, I., G. Segall, D. Birnbaum, A. Ben-Eliyahu, and G. Diesendruck. 2011. Seeing isn’t believing: The effect of intergroup exposure on children’s essentialist beliefs about ethnic categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101: 1139–1156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubriwny, T.N., B.R. Bates, and J.L. Bevan. 2004. Lay understandings of race: Cultural and genetic definitions. Community Genetics 7: 185–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dufour, V., O. Pascalis, and O. Petit. 2006. Face processing limitation to own species in primates: a comparative study in brown capuchins, Tonkean macaques and humans. Behavioural Processes 73: 107–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunham, Y., E.V. Stepanova, R. Dotsch, and A. Todorov. 2014. The development of race-based perceptual categorization: skin color dominates early category judgments. Developmental Science 18 (3): 469–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enos, D.E., and C. Celaya. 2018. The effect of segregation on intergroup relations. Journal of Experimental Political Science 5 (1): 26–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fawcett, C., and B. Tunçgenç. 2017. Infants’ use of movement synchrony to infer social affiliation in others. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 160: 127–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D.M., and C. Holbrook. 2016. Synchronized behaviour increases assessments of the formidability and cohesion of coalitions. Evolution and Human Behavior 37 (6): 502–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freire, A., and K. Lee. 2001. Face recognition in 4- to 7-year-olds: processing of configural, featural, and paraphernalia information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80 (4): 347–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S.A. 2003. The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gil-White, F. 2001. Are ethnic groups biological ‘species’ to the human brain. Current Anthropology 42 (4): 515–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giménez, M., and P.L. Harris. 2010. Understanding constraints on inheritance: Evidence for biological thinking in early childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 20 (3): 307–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glasgow, J. 2009. A theory of race. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goff, P.A., J.L. Eberhardt, M.J. Williams, and M.C. Jackson. 2008. Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94 (2): 292–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannaford, I. 1996. Race: The history of an idea in the West. Washington: Woodrow Wilson Center Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardimon, M. 2003. The ordinary concept of race. The Journal of Philosophy 100 (9): 437–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N., L. Rothschild, and D. Ernst. 2000. Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology 39: 113–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hills, P.J., and M.B. Lewis. 2018. The development of face expertise: Evidence for a qualitative change in processing. Cognitive Development 48: 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L.A. 1993. Discovering social difference: The role of appearance in the development of racial awareness. Cognitive Psychology 25: 317–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L.A. 1995. Do children have a theory of race? Cognition 54: 209–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L.A. 1996. Race in making: Cognition, culture, and the child’s construction of human kinds. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L.A. 2001. On a folk theory of society: Children, evolution, and mental representations of social groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review 5 (2): 107–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L.A., E. Bartmess, S. White, and U. Frith. 2007. Can autistic children predictive behavior by social stereotypes? Current Biology 17 (12): 451–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hochman, A. 2013. Do we need a device to acquire ethnic concepts? Philosophy of Science 80 (5): 994–1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hochman, A. 2019. Is race modern? Disambiguating the question. Du Bois Review: Social Science Research on Race 16 (2): 647–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ip, G.W.M., C.Y. Chiu, and C. Wan. 2006. Birds of a feather and birds flocking together: Physical versus behavioral cues may lead to trait- versus goal-based group perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 368–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T.A., and S. Tomelleri. 2017. Seeing is not stereotyping: the functional independence of categorization and stereotype activation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience: 758–764.

  • Jackson, J.P. 2017. Cognitive/evolutionary psychology and the history of racism. Philosophy of Science 84 (2): 296–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaquet, E., G. Rhodes, and W.G. Hayward. 2007. Opposite aftereffects for Chinese and Caucasian faces are selective for social category information and not just physical face differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 60: 1457–1467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaquet, E., G. Rhodes, and W.G. Hayward. 2008. Race-contingent aftereffects suggest distinct perceptual norms for different race faces. Visual Cognition 16: 734–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalish, C., and C. Lawson. 2008. Development of social category representations: Early appreciation of roles and deontic relations. Child Development 79: 577–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karasawa, M., N. Asai, and K. Hioki. 2019. Psychological essentialism at the explicit and implicit levels: The unique status of social categories. Japanese Psychological Research 61 (2): 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, D., E. Machery, and R. Mallon. 2010. Race and racial cognition. In The Moral Psychology Handbook, ed. J. Doris and the Moral Psychology Reading Group, 433–472. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, D.J., P.C. Quinn, A.M. Slater, K. Lee, L. Ge, and O. Pascalis. 2007. The other-race effect develops during infancy: Evidence of perceptual narrowing. Psychological Science 18: 1084–1089.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, M., V. Macchi Cassia, S. Kanazawa, M.K. Yamaguchi, and R. Kakigi. 2018. Perceptual narrowing towards adult faces is a cross-cultural phenomenon in infancy: A behavioral and near-infrared spectroscopy study with Japanese infants. Developmental Science 21 (1): e12498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, R.S., A.W. Young, M.G. Day, and A.M. Burton. 2017. Robust social categorization emerges from learning the identities of very few faces. Psychological Review 124 (2): 115–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurebayashi, M.A., L.R. Hoffman, C.S. Ryan, and A. Murayama. 2012. Japanese and American perceptions of group entitativity and autonomy: A multilevel analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43: 349–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., J. Tooby, and L. Cosmides. 2001. Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 15387–15392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamer, S.A., T.D. Sweeny, M.L. Dyer, and M. Weisbuch. 2018. Rapid visual perception of interracial crowds: Racial category learning from emotional segregation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 147 (5): 683–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, S.-J. 2014. Carving up the social world with generics. Oxford Studies in Experimental Philosophy 1: 208–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, D.T. 2000. Race as a visual feature: Using visual search and perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the cross-race recognition deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129: 559–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, Z., A.L. Woodward, and K.D. Kinzler. 2017. The origins of social categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21 (7): 556–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lickel, B., D.L. Hamilton, G. Wieczokowska, A. Lewis, S.J. Sherman, and A.N. Uhles. 2000. Varieties of groups and the perception of group entitativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78: 223–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lickel, B., N. Miller, D.M. Stenstrom, T.F. Denson, and T. Schmader. 2006. Vicarious retribution: The role of collective blame in intergroup aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10 (4): 372–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A.C., L.M. DeBruine, and B.C. Jones. 2005. Sex-contingent face aftereffects suggest distinct neural populations code male and female faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 272: 2283–2287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, A.C., L.M. DeBruine, B.C. Jones, and C. Watt. 2008. Category contingent aftereffects for faces of different races, ages and species. Cognition 106: 1537–1547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machery, E., and L. Faucher. 2005. Social construction and the concept of race. Philosophy of Science 72 (5): 1208–1219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machery, E., and L. Faucher. 2017. Why do we think racially? Culture, evolution, and cognition. In Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science, 2nd edition, ed. H. Cohen and C. Lefebvre, 1135–1176. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Machery, E., and L. Faucher. 2020. The folk concept of race. Forthcoming in T. Marques and Å. Wikforss (Eds.), Shifting concepts: The philosophy and psychology of conceptual variability. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Maddox, K.B. 2004. Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Personality and Social Psychology Review 8 (4): 383–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maddox, K.B., and J.M. Perry. 2018. Racial appearance bias: Improving evidence-based policies to address racial disparities. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1): 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mallon, R. 2016. The construction of human kinds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mandalaywala, T., G. Ranger-Murdock, D.M. Amodio, and M. Rhodes. 2019. The nature and consequences of essentialist beliefs about race in early childhood. Child Development 90 (4): e427–e453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKone, E., K. Crookes, and N. Kanwisher. 2009. The cognitive and neural development of face recognition in humans. In M. S. Gazzaniga, E. Bizzi, L. M. Chalupa, S. T. Grafton, T. F. Heatherton, C. Koch, J. E. LeDoux, S. J. Luck, G. R. Mangan, J. A. Movshon, H. Neville, E. A. Phelps, P. Rakic, D. L. Schacter, M. Sur, & B. A. Wandell (Eds.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 467–482). Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • McPherson, M., L. Smith-Lovin, and J.M. Cook. 2001. Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Review of Sociology 27: 415–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meissner, C.A., and J. Brigham. 2001. Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 7 (1): 3–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mondloch, C.J., R. Le Grand, and D. Maurer. 2002. Configural face processing develops more slowly than featural face processing. Perception 31 (5): 553–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moya, C. 2013. Evolved priors for ethnolinguistic categorization: A case study from the Quechua-Aymara boundary in the Peruvian Altiplano. Evolution and Behavior 34: 265–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moya, C., and R. Boyd. 2015. Different selection pressures give rise to distinct ethnic phenomena: A functionalist framework with illustrations from the Peruvian Altiplano. Human Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9224-9.

  • Moya, C., and J. Henrich. 2016. Culture-gene coevolutionary psychology: Cultural learning, language. and ethnic psychology. Current Opinion in Psychology 8: 112–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Toole, A.J., H. Abdi, K.A. Deffenbacher, and J.C. Bartlett. 1991. Classifying faces by race and sex using an autoassociative memory trained for recognition. In Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. K.J. Hammomd and D. Gentner, 847–851. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascalis, O., and J. Bachevalier. 1998. Face recognition in primates: A cross-species study. Behavioural Processes 43: 87–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pauker, K., C. Carpinella, C. Meyers, D.M. Young, and D.T. Sanchez. 2018. The role of diversity exposure in Whites’ reduction in race essentialism over time. Social Psychological and Personality Science 9 (8): 944–952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pauker, K., Y. Xu, A. Williams, and A.M. Biddle. 2016. Race essentialism and social contextual differences in children’s racial stereotyping. Child Development 87: 1409–1422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, B. 2019. The shifting border between perception and cognition. Noûs 53 (2): 316–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, B. 2020. Entitativity and implicit measures of social cognition. Mind & Language. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12350.

  • Phillips, B. 2021. Seeing seeing. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 102 (1): 24–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L.T., M. Weisbuch, and N. Ambady. 2014. People perception: Social vision of groups and consequences for organizing and interacting. Research in Organizational Behavior 34: 101–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L.T., M.L. Slepian, and B.L. Hughes. 2018. Perceiving groups: The people perception of diversity and hierarchy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 114 (5): 766–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pietraszewski, D. 2016. Priming race: Does the mind inhibit categorization by race at encoding or recall? Social Psychological and Personality Science 7 (1): 85–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pietraszewski, D., L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby. 2014. The content of our cooperation, not the color of our skin: An alliance detection system regulates categorization by coalition and race, but not sex. PLoS ONE 9: e88534. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pietraszewski, D., O. Curry, M.B. Petersen, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby. 2015. Constituents of political cognition: Race, party politics, and the alliance detection system. Cognition 140: 24–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, L.J., and E.S. Spelke. 2018. Human infants’ understanding of social imitation: Inferences of affiliation from third-party observations. Cognition 170: 31–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P.C., and P.D. Eimas. 1997. A reexamination of the perceptual-to-conceptual shift in mental representations. Review of General Psychology 1: 271–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. 2012. Naïve theories of social groups. Child Development 83 (6): 1900–1916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. 2013a. The conceptual structure of social categories: The social allegiance hypothesis. In Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 258–262), ed. M.R. Banaji and S.A. Gelman. Oxford University Press.

  • Rhodes, M. 2013b. How two intuitive theories shape the development of social categorization. Child Development Perspectives 7 (1): 12–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M., and S.A. Gelman. 2009. Five-year-olds’ beliefs about the discreteness of category boundaries for animals and artifacts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 16: 920–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M., S.A. Gelman, and J.C. Karuza. 2014. Preschool ontology: The role of beliefs about category boundaries in early categorization. Journal of Cognition and Development 15 (1): 78–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G., T.L. Watson, L. Jeffrey, and C.W.G. Clifford. 2010. Perceptual adaptation helps us identify faces. Vision Research 50: 963–968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, A., P.J. Phillips, V. Natu, X. An, and A.J. O’Toole. 2013. Unaware person recognition from the body when face identification fails. Psychological Science 24 (11): 2235–2243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P.J., and R. Boyd. 2005. Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M., and M. Taylor. 1992. Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds? In Language, interaction and social cognition, ed. G.R. Semin and K. Fiedler, 11–36. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutchick, A.M., D.L. Hamilton, and J.D. Sack. 2008. Antecedents of entitativity in categorically and dynamically construed groups. European Journal of Social Psychology 38: 905–921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, J.L., and J. Glasgow. 2010. Is race‐thinking biological or social, and does it matter for racism? An exploratory study. Social Philosophy 41 (3): 244–259.

  • Shutts, K., C. Roben, and E. Spelke. 2013. Children’s use of social categories in thinking about people and social relationships. Journal of Cognition and Development 14 (1): 35–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedley, A., and B.D. Smedley. 2005. Race as biology is fiction, racism as a social problem is real: Anthropological and historical perspectives on the social construction of race. American Psychologist 60 (1): 16–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D.L. 2011. Less than human: Why we demean, enslave, and exterminate others. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer-Rodgers, J., D.L. Hamilton, and S.J. Sherman. 2007. The central role of entitativity in stereotypes of social categories and task groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92: 369–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sugita, Y. 2008. Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 394–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suhrke, J., C. Freitag, B. Lamm, J. Teiser, I. Fassbender, S. Poloczek, et al. 2014. The other-race effect in 3-year-old German and Cameroonian children. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tham, D.S.Y., and P.J. Woo. 2019. Development of the other-race effect in Malaysian-Chinese infants. Developmental Psychobiology 61 (1): 107–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J.C. 1987. Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, T. 1991. A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Human. Experimental Psychology 43a: 161–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, T., M.B. Lewis, and P.J. Hills. 2016. Face-space: A unifying concept in face recognition research. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (10): 1996–2019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. 2010. Names will never hurt me? Naming and the development of racial and gender categories in preschool-aged children. European Journal of Social psychology 40: 593–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkerson, I. 2020. Caste: The origins of our discontents. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, M.J., and J.L. Eberhardt. 2008. Biological conceptions of race and the motivation to cross racial boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94: 1033–1047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J.E., and J.K. Roberson. 1967. A method for assessing racial attitudes in preschool children. Educational and Psychological Measurement 27 (3): 671–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, S.G., K. Hugenberg, M.J. Bernstein, and D.F. Sacco. 2012. Perception and motivation in face recognition: a critical review of theories of the cross-race effect. Personality and Social Psychology Review 16 (2): 116–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuki, M. 2003. Intergroup comparison versus intragroup relationship: A cross-cultural examination of social identity theory in North American and East Asian cultural contexts. Social Psychology Quarterly 66: 166–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yzerbyt, V.Y., S. Rocher, and G. Schadron. 1997. Stereotypes as explanations: A subjective essentialistic view of group perception. In The psychology of stereotyping and group life, ed. R. Spears, P. Oakes, N. Ellemers, and A. Haslam, 20–50. London: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zack, N. 2002. Philosophy of science and race. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, L., and C. Chubb. 2001. The size-tuning of the face-distortion after-effect. Vision Research 41: 2979–2994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ben Phillips.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Phillips, B. The Roots of Racial Categorization. Rev.Phil.Psych. 13, 151–175 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00525-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00525-w

Navigation