Skip to main content
Log in

Peer interactions of dysphoric adolescents

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study sought to extend our knowledge of dysphoric adolescents' dyadic peer interactions. Thirty female dyads (ages 13–17) were videotaped interacting during both a structured and an unstructured task. Fourteen of the dyads were each comprised of a dysphoric girl and a nondysphoric girl; the remaining dyads were comprised of two nondysphoric girls. Girls were considered dysphoric if they scored 10 or higher on the Beck Depression Inventory. Dysphoric subjects evaluated their performances more negatively than nondysphoric subjects and rated their partners more negatively overall. The partners of the dysphoric girls were viewed by raters as less positive and less happy. They saw the dysphoric teens as making more critical comments and evaluated them more negatively overall. There was also a tendency for these girls to reject their dysphoric partners and to become more passive during the unstructured task. The findings indicate that dysphoric adolescent girls evoke negative reactions from peers and negatively influence their peers' behavior. These negative reactions from others may lead to further difficulties in negotiating the developmental tasks of adolescence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altmann, E. D., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). The social behavior of depressed children: An observational study.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 29–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1987).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Ward, C. M., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. E., & Erbaugh, J. K. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, J., Geller, S., Marton, P., & Kutcher, S. (1992). Peer responses to social interaction with depressed adolescents.Journal of Clinical Child Psychology.21, 365–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C. (1976a). Towards an interactional model of depression.Psychiatry, 39, 28–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C. (1976b). Depression and the response of others.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 186–193.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dembo, M. H., & McAuliffe, T. J. (1987). Effects of perceived ability and grade status on social interaction and influence in cooperative groups.Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 415–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1968).Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Meltzer, S. J. (1987). Depression and the perception of social skill in dyadic interactions.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 41–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., and Robinson, L. A. (1982). Responses to depressed individuals: Discrepancies between self-report and observer-rated behavior.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 231–240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gurtman, M. B. (1986). Depression and the response of others: Re-evaluating the re-evaluation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 99–101.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C. L., & Peters, S. D. (1978). Interpersonal consequences of depression: Responses to men and women enacting a depressed role.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 322–332.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. J. (1990). Effect of interaction goals on expectancy confirmation in a problem-solving context.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 521–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1975).Four factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Kazdin, A. E., Esveldt-Dawson, K., Sherick, R. B., & Colbus, D. (1985). Assessment of overt behavior and childhood depression among psychiatrically disturbed children.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 201–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. C., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991).Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., & Burge, D. (1994). Interpersonal functioning and depressive symptoms in childhood: Addressing the issues of specificity and comorbidity.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 355–371.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segrin, C., & Dillard, J. P. (1992). The interactional theory of depression: A meta-analysis of the research literature.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11, 43–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teri, L. (1982). The use of the Beck Depression Inventory with adolescents.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 277–284.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1965).Manual for the Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist. San Diego: Education and Industrial Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baker, M., Milich, R. & Manolis, M.B. Peer interactions of dysphoric adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol 24, 241–255 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01441630

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01441630

Keywords

Navigation