Skip to main content
Log in

Catching optical information for the regulation of timing

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recent research almost unambiguously refutes the hypothesis that the timing of interceptive actions is solely based on the relative rate of expansion [i.e. τ(ϕ)]. The aim of the present experiment was to evaluated the merits of eight alternative informational variables that recently have been proposed in the literature \( {\left( {{\text{i}}{\text{.e}}{\text{.}}\;\;\ifmmode\expandafter\dot\else\expandafter\.\fi{\varphi }{\text{,}}\;\ifmmode\expandafter\dot\else\expandafter\.\fi{\theta }{\text{,}}\;\ifmmode\expandafter\dot\else\expandafter\.\fi{\Delta }{\text{,}}\;\tau {\left( \varphi \right)}{\text{,}}\;\tau {\left( \theta \right)}{\text{,}}\;\tau {\left( \Delta \right)}{\text{,}}\;\tau (\varphi ,\theta ){\text{,}}\;\zeta } \right)} \). Participants (n=7) were required to regulate the spatio-temporal characteristics of their reach and grasp to catch a ball approaching on a constant spatial trajectory. To identify the information used to regulate the timing of the catch we examined the qualitative effects of ball speed (0.5, 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, and 2.5 m/s) and viewing (monocular versus binocular) on the kinematics of the catch. Subsequently, we directly assessed the quantitative relationship between informational variables and the timing of reach onset and hand closure. The findings raised serious doubts against the use of variables that specified the time-to-contact between the ball and the point of observation (i.e. relative rate of expansion and disparity). Further, optical variables solely confined to the trajectory of the ball (i.e. the absolute rate of expansion) did yield positive results for the timing of reach onset but not for the timing of hand closure. Only variables that were related to the closure of the gap between hand and ball were found to contribute to the timing of hand closure. These results suggest that information related to the constriction of the optical gap between end-effector and ball becomes more important with approach, whereas the contribution of the absolute rate of expansion becomes less leading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1 A
Fig. 2
Fig. 3 A
Fig. 4A,B

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. $$\tau {\left( \varphi \right)} = {{\varphi } \over {{{\mathop \varphi \limits^ \bullet }}}} \approx {{X} \over {{{\mathop X\limits^ \bullet }}}} = TTC_{1} $$

    where X is an organism–environment property (i.e. distance between object and observer), which together with its first time-derivative defines TTC. The subscript 1 indicates that only the first-order time-derivative of X is considered. For small angles of ϕ (e.g. the angle subtended by the edges of the object and the point of observation) the organism–environment property TTC1 is specified by the optical variable τ(ϕ).

  2. Note that the optical variable η, which has received some attention in the perception of collisions as well (Sun and Frost 1998; López-Moliner and Bonnet 2002), is also a joint function of optical angle and rate of expansion.

  3. We investigated whether information sources alone could account for the observed timing of onset patterns, which does of course not exclude the possibility that a regulation based on combinations of information sources may be consistent with our data as well (e.g. Smith et al. 2001; López-Moliner and Bonnet 2002). In the present experiment we cannot falsify such a regulation based on combinations of information sources since the optical variables can be combined with scaling parameters in infinite ways.

References

  • Bennett SJ, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP, Davids K (1999) Timing a one-handed catch I. Effects of telestereoscopic viewing. Exp Brain Res 129:362–368

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett SJ, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP, Davids K (2000) Discriminating the role of binocular information in the timing of a one-handed catch. The effects of telestereoscopic viewing and ball size. Exp Brain Res 135:341–347

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bootsma RJ, Craig CM (2002) Global and local contributions to the optical specification of time-to-contact: observer sensitivity to composite tau. Perception 31:901–924

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bootsma RJ, Oudejans RRD (1993) Visual information about time-to-collision between two objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 19:1041–1052

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bootsma RJ, Van Wieringen PCW (1990) Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:21–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bootsma RJ, Fayt V, Zaal FTJM, Laurent M (1997) On the information-based regulation of movement: what Wann (1996) may want to consider. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:1282–1289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brouwer AM, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2000) Hitting moving objects. The dependency of hand velocity on the speed of the target. Exp Brain Res 133: 242–248

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caljouw SR, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP (2004a) The fallacy of perceiving time-to-contact for the regulation of catching and hitting. In: Hecht H, Savelsbergh GJP (eds) Time-to-contact. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam (in press)

  • Caljouw SR, van der Kamp J., Savelsbergh GJP (2004b) Timing of goal-directed hitting: impact requirements change the information-movement coupling. Exp Brain Res, DOI 10.1007/s00221-003-1705-0

  • Carnahan H, McFayden BJ (1996) Visuomotor control when reaching toward and grasping moving targets. Acta Psychologica 92:17–32

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavallo V, Laurent M (1988) Visual information and skill level in time-to-collision estimation. Perception 17:623–632

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chieffi S, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Gentilucci M (1992) Prehension movements directed to approaching objects: influence of stimulus velocity on the transport and the grasp components. Neuropsychologia 30:877–897

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ (1990) Binocular eye movements and the perception of depth. Rev Oculomot Res 4:213–261

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting JE (1986) Perception with an eye for motion. MIT Press, Cambridge MA

  • DeLucia PR (1991) Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:738–748

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLucia PR, Warren R (1994) Pictorial and motion-based depth information during active control self-motion: size arrival effects on collision avoidance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20:783–789

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dessing JC, Bullock D, Peper CL, Beek PJ (2002) Prospective control of manual interceptive actions: comparative simulations of extant and new model constructs. Neural Netw 15:163–179

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dubrowski A, Carnahan H (2001) Control strategies when intercepting slowly moving targets. J Mot Behav 33:37–48

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch HL, Turvey MT (1978) On the control of activity: some remarks from an ecological point of view. In: Landers D, Christina R (eds) Psychology of motor behavior and sport. Human Kinetics, Champaign IL, pp 3–35

  • Franks IM, Weicker D, Robertson DGE (1985) The kinematics, movement phasing and timing of a skilled action in response to varying conditions of uncertainty. Hum Movem Sci 4:91–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray R, Regan D (1998) Accuracy of estimating time-to-collision using binocular and monocular information. Vision Res 38:499–512

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H (1993) Estimates of time to contact based on changing size and changing target vergence. Perception 22:549–563

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard AW, Seng CN (1954) Visual movements of batters. Res Q 25:42–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Knowles WB, Carel WL (1958) Estimating time to collision. Am Psychol 13:405–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurent M, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJP (1994) The control and coordination of one-handed catching: the effect of temporal constraints. Exp Brain Res 101:314–322

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laurent M, Montagne G, Durey A (1996) Binocular invariants in interceptive tasks: a directed perception approach. Perception 25:1437–1450

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN (1976) A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception 5:437–459

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Reddish DE (1981) Plummeting gannets: a paradigm of ecological optics. Nature 293:293–294

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Young DS, Reddish DE, Lough S, Clayton T M (1983) Visual timing in hitting an accelerating ball. Q J Exp Psychol 35:333–346

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • López-Moliner J, Bonnet C (2002) Speed of response initiations in a time-to-contact discrimination task reflects the use of η. Vision Res 42:2419–2430

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mason AH, Carnahan H (1999) Target viewing time and velocity effects on prehension. Exp Brain Res 127:83–94

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod RW, Ross HE (1983) Optic flow and cognitive factors in time-to-collision. Perception 12:417–423

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michaels CF (2000) Information, perception, and action: what should ecological psychologists learn from Milner and Goodale (1995)? Ecol Psychol 12:241–258

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaels CF, Zeinstra EB, Oudejans RRD (2001) Information and action in punching a falling ball. Q J Exp Psychol, 54:69–93

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Montagne G, Fraisse F, Ripoll H, Laurent M (2000) Perception-action coupling in an interceptive task: first-order time-to-contact as an input variable. Hum Movem Sci 19:59–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peper CL, Bootsma RJ, Mestre DR, Bakker FC (1994) Catching balls: how to get the hand to the right place at the right time. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20:591–612

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purdy WC (1958) The hypothesis of psychophysical correspondence in space perception. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University

  • Regan D, Beverley KI (1979) Binocular and monocular stimuli for motion in depth: changing-disparity and changing-size feed the same motion-in-depth stage. Vision Res 19:1331–1342

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Regan D, Beverley KI, Cynader M (1979) The visual perception of motion in depth. Sci Am 241:136–151

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, Whiting HTA, Bootsma RJ (1991) Grasping tau. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:315–322

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, Whiting HTA, Burden AM, Bartlett RM (1992) The role of predictive visual temporal information in the coordination of muscle activity in catching. Exp Brain Res 89:223–228

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W (1965) Perception of impending collision: A study of visually directed avoidant behavior. Psychol Monogr 79:1–26

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W, Caviness JA, Gibson JJ (1962) Persistent fear responses in rhesus monkeys to the optical stimulus of “looming.” Science 136:982–983

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W, Oldak R, Shah V (1992) Aging persons’ estimates of vehicular motion. Psychol Aging 4:518–525

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA (1982) Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis. Human Kinetics, Champaign IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Sideway B, Fairweather M, Sekiya H, McNitt-Gray J (1996) Time-to-collision estimation in a simulated driving task. Hum Factors 38:101–113

    Google Scholar 

  • Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (1995) Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:19–31

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith MRH, Flach JM, Dittman SM, Stanard T (2001) Monocular optical constraints on collision control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:395–410

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sun HJ, Frost B J (1998) Computation of different optical variables of looming objects in pigeon nucleus rotundus neurons. Nature Neurosci 1:296–303

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR (1994) Approximate information sources and perceptual variables in interceptive timing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20:154–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR (1999) Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? Trends Cogn Sci 3:301–310

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR, Lonergan A (2002) Intercepting a moving target: effects of temporal precision constraints and movement amplitude. Exp Brain Res 142:193–207

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR, Oliver J, Carroll TJ (2003) Temporal precision of interceptive action: differential effects of target size and speed. Exp Brain Res 148:425–438

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tyldesley DA, Whiting HTA (1975) Operational timing. J Hum Movem Stud 1:172–177

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Kamp J (1999) The information-based regulation of interceptive timing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam

  • van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP, Smeets JB (1997) Multiple information sources in interceptive timing. Hum Movem Sci 16:787–821

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kamp J, Bennett SJ, Savelsbergh GJP, Davids K (1999) Timing a one-handed catch II. Adaptation to telestereoscopic viewing. Exp Brain Res 129:369–377

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP, Rosengren KS (2001) The separation of action and perception and the issue of affordances. Ecol Psychol 13:167–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner H (1982) Flow field variables trigger landing in flies. Nature 297:147–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace SA, Stevenson E, Weeks DL, Kelso JAS (1992) The perceptual guidance of grasping a moving object. Hum Movem Sci 11:691–715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wann JP (1996) Anticipating arrival: Is the tau margin a specious theory? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:1031–1048

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH (1988) Action modes and laws of control for the visual guidance of action. In: Meijer OG, Roth K (eds) Complex movement behaviour: ‘The’ motor–action controversy. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, pp 339–379

  • Wing AM, Turton A, Fraser C (1986) Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. J Mot Behav 18:245–260

    Google Scholar 

  • Wollstein JR, Abernethy B (1988) Timing structure in squash strokes—further evidence for the operational timing hypothesis. J Hum Movem Stud 15:61–79

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. R. Caljouw.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Caljouw, S.R., van der Kamp, J. & Savelsbergh, G.J.P. Catching optical information for the regulation of timing. Exp Brain Res 155, 427–438 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1739-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1739-3

Keywords

Navigation