Skip to main content
Log in

Cooperation or competition? Discriminating between social intentions by observing prehensile movements

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Body movement provides a rich source of information about other people’s goals and intentions. In the present study, we examined a particular aspect concerned with the interpretation of bodily movement—how well people can distinguish between different social intentions by observing a reach-to-grasp movement. To ascertain to what extent intention-from-motion judgements rely on visual kinematics, we compared prediction performance on a typical temporal-occlusion video task with prediction performance on a temporal-occlusion point-light task. In the video task, participants observed videos showing a model reaching towards and grasping a wooden block with different intents: to cooperate with a partner in building a tower, compete with an opponent to be the first to put the object in the middle of the working surface, or perform an individual action. In the point-light task, participants observed point-light displays of the same movements. Although predictions were more accurate for the video task, prediction performance was not disrupted for the point-light task, suggesting that essential kinematic information available in point-light displays was indeed sufficient for intention-from-motion judgement. Importantly, the same kinematic landmarks were used to discriminate between social intentions for the video and the point-light task. This suggests that observers not only have the ability to use kinematic information when no other information is available, but they use kinematic information to discriminate between intentions when watching the motion of others under full light conditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abernethy B, Zawi K (2007) Pickup of essential kinematics underpins expert perception of movement patterns. J Mot Behav 39:353–367

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abernethy B, Gill DP, Parks SL, Packer ST (2001) Expertise and the perception of kinematic and situational probability information. Perception 30:233–252

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Abernethy B, Zawi K, Jackson RC (2008) Expertise and attunement to kinematic constraints. Perception 37:931–948

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aglioti SM, Cesari P, Romani M, Urgesi C (2008) Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nat Neurosci 11:1109–1116

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ansuini C, Santello M, Massaccesi S, Castiello U (2006) Effects of end-goal on hand shaping. J Neurophysiol 95:2456–2465

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ansuini C, Giosa L, Turella L, Altoe G, Castiello U (2008) An object for an action, the same object for other actions: effects on hand shaping. Exp Brain Res 185:111–119

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson AP, Dittrich WH, Gemmell AJ, Young AW (2004) Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception 33:717–746

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio C, Sartori L, Bulgheroni M, Castiello U (2008a) The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: a kinematic study on social intention. Conscious Cogn 17:557–564

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio C, Sartori L, Bulgheroni M, Castiello U (2008b) Both your intention and mine are reflected in the kinematics of my reach-to-grasp movement. Cognition 106:894–912

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio C, Sartori L, Castiello U (2010) Towards you: the social side of actions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19:183–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore S-J, Frith CD (2005) The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action. Neuropsychologia 43:260–267

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino B, Glaser DE, Grèzes J, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb Cortex 15:1243–1249

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chaminade T, Hodgins J, Kawato M (2007) Anthropomorphism influences perception of computer-animated characters’ actions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2:206–216

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke TJ, Bradshaw MF, Field DT, Hampson SE, Rose D (2005) The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception 34:1171–1180

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dittrich WH (1993) Action categories and the perception of biological motion. Perception 22:15–22

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dittrich WH, Troscianko T, Lea SE, Morgan D (1996) Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception 25:727–738

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith CD, Frith U (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50:531–534

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou I, Becchio C, Glover S, Castiello U (2007) Different action patterns for cooperative and competitive behaviour. Cognition 102:415–433

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grezes J, Frith C, Passingham RE (2004) Brain mechanisms for inferring deceit in the actions of others. J Neurosci 24:5500–5505

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heeger D (1997) Signal detection theory. Published online: http://www.cns.nyu.edu/~david/handouts/sdt-advanced.pdf

  • Kilner JM, Vargas C, Duval S, Blakemore S-J, Sirigu A (2004) Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement. Nat Neurosci 7:1299–1301

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich G, Flach R (2001) Predicting the effects of actions: interactions of perception and action. Psychol Sci 12:467–472

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan NA, Creelman CD (2005) Detection theory: a user’s guide, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan NA, Kaplan HL (1985) Detection theory analysis of group data: estimating sensitivity from average hit and false-alarm rates. Psychol Bull 98:185–199

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pollick FE, Paterson HM, Bruderlin A, Sanford AJ (2001) Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition 82:B51–B61

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pollick FE, Lestou V, Ryu J, Cho SB (2002) Estimating the efficiency of recognizing gender and affect from biological motion. Vision Res 42:2345–2355

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz W (1997) Perception and action planning. Eur J Cogn Psychol 9:129–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz W (2006) What re-enactment earns us. Cortex 42:515–517

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C (2010) The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:264–274

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Runeson S, Frykholm G (1983) Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. J Exp Psychol Gen 112:585–615

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sartori L, Becchio C, Castiello U (2011) Cues to intention: the role of movement information. Cognition 119:242–252

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sartori L, Becchio C, Bara BG, Castiello U (2009) Does the intention to communicate affect action kinematics? Conscious Cogn 18:766–772

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schütz-Bosbach S, Prinz W (2007) Perceptual resonance. Action-induced modulation of perception. Trends in Cogn Sci 11:349–355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz N, Shiffrar M (2009) Detecting deception in a bluffing body: the role of expertise. Psychon Bull Rev 16:170–175

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shim J, Carlton LG, Chow JW, Chae W (2005) The use of anticipatory visual cues by highly skilled tennis players. J Mot Behav 37:164–175

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà MA, Kohler E, Gallese V, Fogassi L, Fadiga L, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2001) I know what you are doing. A neurophysiological study. Neuron 31:155–165

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vanrie J, Verfaillie K (2004) Perception of biological motion: a stimulus set of human point-light actions. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 36:625–629

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vingerhoets G, Honore P, Vandekerckhove E, Nys J, Vandemaele P, Achten E (2010) Multifocal intraparietal activation during discrimination of action intention in observed tool grasping. Neuroscience 169:1158–1167

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson M, Knoblich G (2005) The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychol Bull 131:460–473

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wöllner C, Cañal-Bruland R (2010) Keeping an eye on the violinist: motor experts show superior timing consistency in a visual perception task. Psychol Res 74:579–585

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2001) Motor prediction. Curr Biol 11:R729–R732

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Umberto Castiello.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Manera, V., Becchio, C., Cavallo, A. et al. Cooperation or competition? Discriminating between social intentions by observing prehensile movements. Exp Brain Res 211, 547–556 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2649-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2649-4

Keywords

Navigation