Skip to main content
Log in

Keep it Coherent: A Meta-Analysis of the Seductive Details Effect

  • Meta-Analysis
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Studies have shown that learners exposed to interesting and irrelevant information, known as seductive details, do not perform as much as those who learned without seductive details. However, findings are mixed in terms of the degree to which seductive details hinder learning. Further research is also needed on how design features of learning materials influence the seductive details effect. This meta-analysis summarizes the seductive details effect and investigates the moderating factors of design and methodology. We also discuss evidence supporting each of the four hypothetical underlying mechanisms for the seductive details effect. Findings show that including seductive details in learning material can hinder learning. Mean effect sizes were moderated by the presence of seductive details, image type used in comparison, delivery format, language, subject, learner pacing, recall question type, and manipulation check approach. We conclude by highlighting limitations in current research, suggesting opportunities for future research, and examining practical implications.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

Studies included in meta-analysis are marked with an asterisk (*)

  • *Abercrombie, S. (2013). Transfer effects of adding seductive details to case-based instruction. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 149–157.

  • *Abercrombie, S., Hushman, C. J., & Carbonneau, K. J. (2019). The impact of seductive details and signaling on analogical transfer. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3491.

  • Adesope, O. O., Trevisan, D. A., & Sundararajan, N. (2017). Rethinking the use of tests: A meta-analysis of practice testing. Review of Educational Research87, 659.

  • Ainsworth, S. (2006). DeFT: A conceptual framework for considering learning with multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 16, 183–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Ataya, R. L. (2003). Knowledge, interest, and reading comprehension: A general linear model for social studies. Storrs: University of Connecticut.

  • Beckmann, J. (2010). Taming a beast of burden—On some issues with the conceptualisation and operationalisation of cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 20, 250–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 1088–1101.

  • Bloom, B. S., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives. The classification of educational goals, Handbook I: Cognitive domain. New York: Longmans Green.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S., Madaus, G. F., & Hastings, J. T. (1981). Evaluation to improve learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. R. (2015). Introduction to meta-analysis. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. C., Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger III, H. L. (2007). The effect of type and timing of feedback on learning from multiple-choice tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13(4), 273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castro-Alonso, J. C., Ayres, P., Wong, M., & Paas, F. (2018). Learning symbols from permanent and transient visual presentations: Don't overplay the hand. Computers & Education, 116, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, Y., & Choi, S. (2014). Effects of seductive details evidenced by gaze duration. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 109, 131–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, O., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2017). The expertise reversal effect is a variant of the more general element interactivity effect. Educational Psychology Review, 29, 393–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Choi, S. (2006). Seductive details and concrete elaboration and their effects on the 1styear EFL high school students’ written text comprehension and interest. Buffalo: State University of New York at Buffalo.

  • Cooper, H. M., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. (Eds.). (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315(7109), 629–634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eitel, A., & Kühl, T. (2019). Harmful or helpful to learning? The impact of seductive details on learning and instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 3–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eitel, A., Bender, L., & Renkl, A. (2019). Are seductive details seductive only when you think they are relevant? An experimental test of the moderating role of perceived relevance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 20–30. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagerland, M. W. (2015). Evidence-based medicine and systematic reviews. In Research in Medical and Biological Sciences (2nd ed., pp. 431–461). https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-799943-2.00012-4.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • *Fries, L., DeCaro, M., & Ramirez, G. (2019). The lure of seductive details during lecture learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111, 736–750. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000301.

  • *Garner, R., Gillingham, M. G., & White, C. S. (1989). Effects of ‘seductive details’ on macroprocessing and microprocessing in adults and children. Cognition and Instruction, 6, 41–57.

  • Ginns, P. (2006). Integrating information: A meta-analysis of the spatial contiguity and temporal contiguity effects. Learning and Instruction, 16, 511–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Hansen, E. J. (2014). How are curious people affected by seductive details in written texts? Exploring the various relationships among curiosity, the seductive details effect, and learning outcomes. Dekalb: Northern Illinois University.

  • *Harp, S. F., & Maslich, A. A. (2005). The consequences of including seductive details during lecture. Teaching of Psychology, 32, 100–103.

  • *Harp, S. F., & Mayer, R. E. (1998). How seductive details do their damage: A theory of cognitive interest in science learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 50, 414–434.

  • Hedges L. V., Olkin I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego: Academic.

  • Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep41024.

  • *Jaeger, A. J., & Wiley, J. (2014). Do illustrations help or harm metacomprehension accuracy?. Learning and Instruction, 34, 58–73.

  • *Jaeger, A. J., Velazquez, M. N., Dawdanow, A., & Shipley, T. F. (2018). Sketching and summarizing to reduce memory for seductive details in science text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 899–916. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000254.

  • *Johnston, G. S. (2002). Effects of seductive and boring details on readers’ comprehension of explanatory texts. Lexington: University of Kentucky.

  • Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory: How many types of load does it really need? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S. (2014). The expertise reversal principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd, rev ed., pp. 576–597). New York: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139547369.028.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., & Renkl, A. (2010). Expertise reversal effect and its instructional implications: Introduction to the special issue. Instructional Science, 38, 209–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Ketzer-Nöltge, A., Schweppe, J., & Rummer, R. (2019). Is the seductive details effect moderated by mood? An eye tracking study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 62–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3487.

  • Kirschner, P. A., Ayres, P., & Chandler, P. (2011). Contemporary cognitive load theory research: The good, the bad and the ugly. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 99–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Korbach, A., Brünken, R., & Park, B. (2016). Learner characteristics and information processing in multimedia learning: A moderated mediation of the seductive details effect. Learning and Individual Differences, 51, 59–68.

  • *Korbach, A., Brünken, R., & Park, B. (2017). Measurement of cognitive load in multimedia learning: A comparison of different objective measures. Instructional Science, 45, 515–536.

  • Krathwohl, D. R. (2002). A revision of Bloom's taxonomy: An overview. Theory into Practice, 41, 212–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Kühl, T., Moersdorf, F., Römer, M., & Münzer, S. (2019). Adding emotionality to seductive details—Consequences for learning? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 48–61. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3477.

  • Lehman, S., Schraw, G., McCrudden, M. T., & Hartley, K. (2007). Processing and recall of seductive details in scientific text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 569–587.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Lehmann, J. A., & Seufert, T. (2017). The influence of background music on learning in the light of different theoretical perspectives and the role of working memory capacity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1902.

  • *Lehmann, J., Hamm, V., & Seufert, T. (2019). The influence of background music on learners with varying extraversion: Seductive detail or beneficial effect? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 85–93. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3509.

  • *Lenzner, A., Schnotz, W., & Müller, A. (2013). The role of decorative pictures in learning. Instructional Science, 41, 811–831.

  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis: Applied social research methods series (Vol. 49). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lusk, D. L. (2008). The effects of seductive details and segmentation on interest, recall and transfer in a multimedia learning environment. Blacksburg: Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Magner, U. I., Schwonke, R., Aleven, V., Popescu, O., & Renkl, A. (2014). Triggering situational interest by decorative illustrations both fosters and hinders learning in computer-based learning environments. Learning and Instruction, 29, 141–152.

  • Mayer, R. E. (2011). Applying the science of learning to multimedia instruction. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 55, pp. 77–108). Academic Press.

  • Mayer, R. E. (2014). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed., pp. 43–71). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Wittrock, M. C. (1996). Problem-solving transfer. Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 47–62).

  • Mayer, R. E., & Fiorella, L. (2014). Principles for reducing extraneous processing in multimedia learning: Coherence, signaling, redundancy, special contiguity, and temporal contiguity principles. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 279–315). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Pilegard, C. (2014). Principles for managing essential processing in multimedia learning: segmenting, pre-training, and modality principles. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 316–344). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., Steinhoff, K., Bower, G., & Mars, R. (1995). A generative theory of textbook design: Using annotated illustrations to foster meaningful learning of science text. Educational Technology Research and Development, 43, 31–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 187–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., Dow, G. T., & Mayer, S. (2003). Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: What works in the design of agent-based microworlds? Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 806.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrudden, M. T. (2019). The effect of task relevance instructions on memory for text with seductive details. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 31–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • *McCrudden, M. T., & Corkill, A. J. (2010). Verbal ability and the processing of scientific text with seductive detail sentences. Reading Psychology, 31, 282–300.

  • *Mensink, M.C. (2011). The influence of prereading and recall instructions on attention and memory for scientific seductive text. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

  • Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. E. (2000). A coherence effect in multimedia learning: The case for minimizing irrelevant sounds in the design of multimedia instructional messages. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesbit, J. C., & Adesope, O. O. (2006). Learning with concept and knowledge maps: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research76, 413–448.

  • Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Statistics, 8, 157–159.

  • *Ozdemir, D., & Doolittle, P. (2015). Revisiting the seductive details effect in multimedia learning: Context-dependency of seductive details. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 24, 101–119.

  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Park, B., & Brünken, R. (2015). The rhythm method: A new method for measuring cognitive load—An experimental dual-task study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 232–243.

  • Park, S., & Lim, J. (2007). Promoting positive emotion in multimedia learning using visual illustrations. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 16, 141–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, S., Kim, M., Lee, Y., Son, C., & Lee, M. (2005). The effects of visual illustrations on learners’ achievement and interest in PDA- (Personal Digital Assistant) based learning. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 33, 173–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, B., Moreno, R., Seufert, T., & Brünken, R. (2011). Does cognitive load moderate the seductive details effect? A multimedia study. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 5–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Park, B., Flowerday, T., & Brünken, R. (2015a). Cognitive and affective effects of seductive details in multimedia learning. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 267–278.

  • *Park, B., Korbach, A., & Brünken, R. (2015b). Do learner characteristics moderate the seductive-details-effect? A cognitive-load-study using eye-tracking. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 18, 24.

  • Peduzzi, P., Concato, J., Feinstein, A. R., & Holford, T. R. (1995). Importance of events per independent variable in proportional hazards regression analysis II. Accuracy and precision of regression estimates. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 48(12), 1503–1510.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rey, G. D. (2011). Seductive details in multimedia messages. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 20, 61–92.

  • Rey, G. D. (2012). A review of research and a meta-analysis of the seductive detail effect. Educational Research Review, 7, 216–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rey, G. D. (2014). Seductive details and attention distraction–an eye tracker experiment. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 133–144.

  • Rey, G. D., Beege, M., Nebel, S., Wirzberger, M., Schmitt, T. H., & Schneider, S. (2019). A meta-analysis of the segmenting effect. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 389–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richter, J., Scheiter, K., & Eitel, A. (2016). Signaling text-picture relations in multimedia learning: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 17, 19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rodenberg, B. R. (2001). The effects of seductive details on students’ understanding of content in a middle school science activity. College Park: University of Maryland.

  • Rop, G., Verkoeijen, P. P., & van Gog, T. (2017). With task experience students learn to ignore the content, not just the location of irrelevant information. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29, 599–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem” and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638.

  • Rowland, E., Skinner, C. H., Davis-Richards, K., Saudargas, R., & Robinson, D. H. (2008). An investigation of placement and type of seductive details: The primacy effect of seductive details on text recall. Research in the Schools, 15, 80–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rowland-Bryant, E. K., Skinner, C. H., Skinner, A. L., Saudargas, R., Robinson, D. H., & Kirk, E. R. (2009). Investigating the interaction of graphic organizers and seductive details:Can a graphic organizer mitigate the seductive-details effect? Research in the Schools, 16, 29–40.

  • Sanchez, C. A., & Wiley, J. (2006). An examination of the seductive details effect in terms of working memory capacity. Memory and Cognition, 34(2), 344–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., & Heise, E. (2014). Distraction during learning with hypermedia: Difficult tasks help to keep task goals on track. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 268.

  • *Schneider, S., Wirzberger, M., & Rey, D. (2019). The moderating role of arousal on the seductive detail effect in a multimedia learning setting. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 71–84. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3473.

  • *Schneider, S., Nebel, S., & Rey, G. D. (2016). Decorative pictures and emotional design in multimedia learning. Learning and Instruction, 44, 65–73.

  • Schneider, S., Dyrna, J., Meier, L., Beege, M., & Rey, G. D. (2017). How affective charge and text–picture connectedness moderate the impact of decorative pictures on multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000209.

  • Schneider, S., Beege, M., Nebel, S., & Rey, G. D. (2018). A meta-analysis of how signaling affects learning with media. Educational Research Review, 23, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W. (2005). An integrated model of text and picture comprehension. The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, 49, 69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G. (1998). Processing and recall differences among selective details. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 3–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Lehman, S. (2001). Situational interest: A review of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), 23–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder, N. L., & Cenkci, A. T. (2018). Spatial contiguity and spatial split-attention effects in multimedia learning environments: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 679–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Rodgers, J. L. (2019). Psychology, science, and knowledge construction: Broadening perspectives from the replication crisis. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 487–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Sundararajan, N. (2018). Seductive details: A meta-regression and empirical study. Doctoral Dissertation, Washington State University.

  • *Sung, E., & Mayer, R. E. (2012). When graphics improve liking but not learning from online lessons. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1618–1625.

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1991). Evidence for cognitive load theory. Cognition and Instruction, 8, 351–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Van Merrienboer, J. J., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 261–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thalheimer, W. (2004). Bells, whistles, neon, and purple prose: When interesting words, sounds, and visuals hurt learning and performance-a review of the seductive-augmentation research.

  • *Tislar, C.L (2017). Investigating the importance of detail interest level and learning objectives on the seductive detail effect. Houghton: Michigan Technological University.

  • Towler, A., Kraiger, K., Sitzmann, T., Van Overberghe, C., Cruz, J., Ronen, E., et al. (2008). The seductive details effect in technology-delivered instruction. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 21, 65–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, M.-. J., Wu, A.-. H., & Chen, J.-. P. (2019). Static and dynamic seductive illustration effects on text‐and‐graphic learning processes: Evidence from eye‐tracking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 108–122. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3514.

  • Tulving, E., & Kroll, N. (1995). Novelty assessment in the brain and long-term memory encoding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2(3), 387–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Merrienboer, J. J., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177.

  • Wang, Z., & Adesope, O. (2014) Effects of seductive details on multimedia learning. Journal of Studies in Education, 4(3), 32–44.

  • *Wang, Z., & Adesope, O. (2016). Does learners' prior knowledge moderate the detrimental effects of seductive details in reading from text? A 2 by 3 study. International Journal of Instruction9, 35–50.

  • *Wang, Z., Sundararajan, N., Adesope, O. O., & Ardasheva, Y. (2017). Moderating the seductive details effect in multimedia learning with note‐taking. British Journal of Educational Technology, 48, 1380–1389.

  • *Wang, Z., Ardasheva, Y. & Lin, L. (2019). Does high perceptual load assist in reducing the seductive details effect? Educational Psychology https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2019.1686465.

  • Wiley, J., Sarmento, D., & Griffin, T. (2019). Picture this! Effects of photographs, diagrams, animations, and sketching on learning and beliefs about learning from a geoscience text. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 9–19. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Wirzberger, M., & Rey, G. D. (2018). Attention please! Enhanced attention control abilities compensate for instructional impairments in multimedia learning. Journal of Computers in Education, 5, 243–257.

  • *Yoo, A (2016). Emotional and cognitive interest: How creating situational interest affects learning with multimedia. Atlanta: Georgia Institute of Technology.

  • *Yue, C. L., & Bjork, E. L. (2017). Using selective redundancy to eliminate the seductive details effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31, 565–571.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to NarayanKripa Sundararajan.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sundararajan, N., Adesope, O. Keep it Coherent: A Meta-Analysis of the Seductive Details Effect. Educ Psychol Rev 32, 707–734 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09522-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09522-4

Keywords

Navigation