Skip to main content
Log in

Bayesian Rationality Revisited: Integrating Order Effects

  • Published:
Foundations of Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Bayes’ inference cannot reliably account for uncertainty in mental processes. The reason is that Bayes’ inference is based on the assumption that the order in which the relevant features are evaluated is indifferent, which is not the case in most of mental processes. Instead of Bayes’ rule, a more general, probabilistic rule of inference capable of accounting for these order effects is established. This new rule of inference can be used to improve the current Bayesian models of cognition. Moreover, it should play an essential role in the search for artificial emotional intelligence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Reducing mental processes to brain’s activity leads to the idea that Bayesian inference “takes part of the automatic and unconscious, elementary operations of our brain” (Dehaene, 2012). According to a growing trend in theoretical neuroscience, the human perceptual system could thus be modeled as a Bayesian machine. The brain is supposed to represent sensory information probabilistically, in the form of probability distributions. This hypothesis, which presently lacks of experimental confirmation (Knill & Pouget, 2004), is only an over-interpretation of Bayesian rationalism within a reductionist materialism. It will not be examined further in this article, which focuses on the rationality of mental processes.

  2. It is supposed that the drawings are equiprobable.

  3. The numbers between brackets are the standard deviations.

  4. Indeed, the target emotional state is not totally reached by the subject. The distinction between the latter state and her real emotional state is considered in the more precise calculations of Sect. 5 and Appendix 1.

  5. The pre-ejection period is the time elapsed between the depolarization of the left ventricle and the beginning of ventricular ejection. Its value is strongly dependent on that of the volume of blood ejected by the left ventricle at each cardiac cycle.

  6. The precise calculation of the relevant commutators and probabilities for this survey will not be presented here since we essentially focus on the order effects involved in cognitive processes (and not in decision making).

  7. Also note that another characterization of this order effect has been proposed more recently by Busemeyer & Wang (2017), by referring to a so-called “ABA experiment”, where a measurement of B is inserted between two measurements of A. It shows that the second measurement of A can be different from the initial one and the degree of incompatibility of A and B can thus be evaluated from this difference. This order effect has been checked on the sequence ABA with a population of 325 participants on a wide range of 12 different set of issues.

  8. The Facial Action Coding System has been developed by the psychologists Paul Ekman and Wallace Friesen in 1978. It is now the standard tool used in psycho-physiological studies of facial expression.

References

  • Aerts, D., Sozzo, S., Gabora, L., & Veloz, T. (2011). Quantum structure in cognition: Fundamentals and applications. In ICQNM 2011: The fifth international conference on quantum, nano and micro technologies.

  • Aerts, D., & Sozzo, S. (2013). Quantum structure in economics: The Ellsberg paradox. ArXiv: https://arxiv.org/abs/1301.0751 v1 [physics.soc-ph] 4 Jan 2013.

  • Ashby, F., & Alfonso-Reese, L. (1995). Categorization as probability density estimation. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 39, 216–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atmanspacher, H., & Römer, H. (2012). Order effects in sequential measurements of non-commuting psychological observables. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56, 274–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bachorowski, J. A., & Braaten, E. B. (1994). Emotional intensity: Measurement and theoretical implications. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 191–199.

  • Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., & Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 20(1), 1–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broemeling, L. D. (2011). Bayesian methods for medical test accuracy. Diagnostics, 1(1), 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Busemeyer, J. R., & Bruza, P. (2012). Quantum models of cognition and decision. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Busemeyer, J., & Wang, Z. (2017). Is there a problem with quantum models of psychological measurements? Plos One, 8, 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1930). The subjective character of cognition and the pre-sensational development of perception. British Journal of Psychology, 14, 166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, I., Sebe, N., Gozman, F.G., Cirelo, M.C., & Huang, T.S. (2003). Learning Bayesian network classifiers for facial expression recognition both labeled and unlabeled data. In 2003 IEEE computer society conference on computer vision and pattern recognition, proceedings 2003.

  • Cox, R. T. (1946). Probability, frequency, and reasonable expectation. American Journal of Physics, 14, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cruz, N., Baratgin, J., Oaksford, M., & Over, D. (2015). Bayesian reasoning with ifs and ands and ors. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 192.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Finetti, B. (1970). Logical foundations and measurement of subjective probability. Acta Psychologica, 34, 129–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2012). Course on the Bayesian brain. https://www.college-de-france.fr/media/stanislas-dehaene/UPL5981541562447201298_Cours2012_CerveauStatisticien_6.pdf.

  • Duran, J. I., & Fernandez-Dols J-M. (2018). Do emotions result in their predicted facial expressions? A meta-analysis of studies on the link between expression and emotion. PsyArXiv : https://psyarxiv.com/65qp7.

  • Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 205–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsberg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity and the savage axioms. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 75(4), 643–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J., Thompson, V., & Over, D. (2015). Uncertain deduction and conditional reasoning. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1909). Cinq leçons sur la psychanalyse, deuxième leçon : Résistance et refoulement. In Leçons d'introduction à la psychanalyse, Presses Universitaires de France, « Quadrige », 2013, p. 297–312.

  • Geringer, J. M., Madsen, K. M., & Gregory, D. (2004). A fifteen-year history of the continuous response digital interface: Issues relating to validity and reliability. Bulletin of the Council for Research in MusicEducation, 160, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleason, A. M. (1957). Measures on the closed subspaces of a hilbert space. Journal of Mathematics and Mechanics, 6, 885–893.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, T. L., Sanborn, A., Canini, K., & Navarro, D. (2008). Categorization as nonparameteric Bayesian density estimation. In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), The probabilistic mind: Prospects for Bayesian cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, L., Kemp, T., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2008a). Bayesian models of cognition. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pishro-Nik H. (2014). Introduction to probability, statistics, and random processes. https://www.probabilitycourse.com/.

  • How, C. Q., Lotto, R. B., & Purves, D. (2006). Comparison of Bayesian and empirical ranking approaches to visual perception. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 241, 866–875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hwang, H., & Matsumoto, D. (2021). Functions of emotions. In R. Biswas-Diener & E. Diener (Eds.), Noba textbook series: Psychology. Champaign: DEF publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassam, K. S., & Mendes, W. B. (2013). The effects of measuring emotion: Physiological reactions to emotional situations depend on whether someone is asking. Plos One, 8(6), e64959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kersten, D., & Yuille, A. (2003). Bayesian models of object perception. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 150–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klement, R. J., & Bandyopadhyay, P. S. (2020). The Epistemology of a positive SARS-CoV-2 test. Acta Biotheoretica. 1–17

  • Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 712–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreibig, S. D., Wilhelm, F. H., Roth, W., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Cardiovascular, electrodermal, and respiratory response patterns to fear- and sadness-induced films. Psychophysiology, 44, 787–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kubota, T. (2017). Artificial intelligence used to identify skin cancer. http://news.stanford.edu/2017/01/25/artificial-intelligence-used-identify-skin-cancer/.

  • Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, S., & Deng, W. (2018). Deep facial expression recognition: A Survey. Computer Science Psycxhology. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 99.

  • Linden, M., & d’Argembeau, A. (1999). L'émotion, ciment du souvenir. Cerveau et Psycho, 28.

  • Mamassian, P., Landy, M., & Maloney, L. T. (2002). Bayesian modelling of visual perception in: Rao R.P.N. probabilistic models of the brain: Perception and neural function. MIT Press, 2002, 13–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. W. (2002). Measuring new types of question order effects. Public Opinion Quarterly, 66(1), 80–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, J. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification: Categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 115(1), 39–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2007). Bayesian rationality: The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pauls, C. A., & Stemmler, G. (2003). Repressive and defensive coping during fear and anger. Emotion, 3(3), 284–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfors, A., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2009). Learning to learn categories. In N. Taatgen, H. van Rijn, L. Schomaker, & J. Nerbonne (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 136–141). Austin, TX: Cognitive science society.

  • Perfors, A., Tenenbaum, J. B., Griffiths, T. L., & Xu, F. (2011). A tutorial introduction to Bayesian models of cognitive development. Cognition, 120(3), 302–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poggio, T. (2016). Deep learning: Mathematics and neuroscience. A Sponsored Supplement to Science, brain-inspired intelligent robotics: The intersection of robotics and neuroscience: 9–12.

  • Poria, S., Cambria, E., Bajpai, R., & Hussain, A. (2017). A review of affective computing: From unimodal analysis to multimodal fusion. Information Fusion, 37, 98–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pothos, E. M., & Busemeyer, J. R. (2009). A quantum probability explanation for violations of “rational” decision theory. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 2171–2178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prkachin, K. M., Williams-Avery, R. M., Zwaal, C., & Mills, D. E. (1999). Cardiovascular changes during induced emotion: An application of Lang’s theory of emotional imagery. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47(3), 255–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E. (2007). Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 37–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roiser, J. P., & Sahakian, B. J. (2013). Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectrums, 18(3), 139–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santhoshkumar, R., & Geetha, M. (2019). Deep learning approach for emotion recognition from human body movements with feedforward deep convolution neural networks. Procedia Computer Science, 152, 158–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, L. J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonyan, K., & Zisserman, A. (2015). Very deep convolutional networks for large-scale image recognition. Computer Science, 340–352.

  • Singh, M., Majumder, A., & Behera, L. (2014). (2014) Facial expressions recognition system using Bayesian inference. International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (IJCNN), 2014, 1502–1509. https://doi.org/10.1109/IJCNN.2014.6889754

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, R., Lovallo, W. R., & Parsons, O. A. (1992). Cardiovascular differentiation of emotions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 422–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stolorow, R. D. (2005). The contextuality of emotional experience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 22(1), 101–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uzan, P. (2014). Psychologie cognitive et calcul quantique. Implications Philosophiques, section Epistémologie, Avril 2014.

  • Uzan, P. (2016). Complementarity in psychophysics. In H. Atmanspacher, T. Filk, & E. Pothos (Eds.), Lecture notes in computer science. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Q., & Ross, M. (2007). Culture and memory. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 645–667). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Z., & Busemeyer, J. R. (2013). A Quantum question order model supported by empirical tests of an a priori and precise prediction. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5(4), 689–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Z., Solloway, T., Shiffrin, R. M., & Busemeyer, J. M. (2014). Context effects produced by question orders reveal quantum nature of human judgments. PNAS, 111(26), 9431–9436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Word learning as Bayesian inference: Evidence from preschoolers. Psychological Review, 114(2), 245–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pierre Uzan.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

There is no conflict of interest of any kind.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 40 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Uzan, P. Bayesian Rationality Revisited: Integrating Order Effects. Found Sci 28, 507–528 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10699-022-09838-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10699-022-09838-0

Keywords

Navigation