Skip to main content
Log in

Taking your mind for a walk: a qualitative investigation of walking and thinking among nine Norwegian academics

  • Published:
Higher Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Walking has long been associated with thinking. Anecdotal evidence from philosophers, writers, researchers, artists, business leaders and so forth testify to the powers of walking-for-thinking. This study explores walking-for-thinking among nine academics in Norway, four university professors, two research and development professionals, two researchers and a university president, who utilize walking-for-thinking as an explicit practice in the their professional lives. Based on in-depth semi-structured qualitative interviews, the study identifies walking-for-thinking as a specific form of walking that has a steady rhythm and a specific individual speed that is experienced as most conducive to thinking. Further, the subjects experience walking-for-thinking as moving gestalt, an interplay between the person, environment and thinking where the rhythms of the body correlate with the rhythm of walking, affording feeling of enhanced memory and creativity. It is suggested that walking-for-thinking should be regarded as a an alternative space for inquiry especially today when sitting has been identified as an independent health hazard and when sedentary workers are urged to look for alternative ways of working that include more movement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This finding is further explored in another paper.

References

  • Andrade, J. (2010). What does doodling do? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 100–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aspinall, P., Mavros, P., Coyne, R., & Roe, J. (2013). The urban brain: Analysing outdoor physical activity with mobile EEG. British Journal of Sports Medicine,. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2012-091877.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atchley, R. A., Strayer, D. L., & Atchley, P. (2012). Creativity in the wild: Improving creative reasoning through immersion in natural settings. PLoS One, 7(12), e51474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Athamatten, E. (2012). Philosophy begins with wander. Nomadic Sojourns Journal, 2, 13–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1117–1122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brisswalter, J., Durand, M., Delignieres, D., & Legros, P. (1995). Optimal and non-optimal demand in a dual task of pedaling and simple reaction time: Effects on energy expenditure and cognitive performance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 29(1), 15–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin,. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 499–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture, and person: A systems view of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 325–339). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity, flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, T. H. (2005). Thinking for a living. Boston: Harvard Business Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, B. M., & Gronlund, S. D. (1998). Task interruption and its effects on memory. Memory, 6(6), 665–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 3017–3022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferris, L. T., Williams, J. S., & Shen, C. L. (2007). The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 39(4), 728–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flaherty, A. W. (2005). Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 147–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, A. B., Rosen, J., & Crawford, M. (2009). Distractions, distractions: Does instant messaging affect college students’ performance on a concurrent reading comprehension task? CyberPsychology and Behavior, 12(1), 51–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds: An anatomy of creativity seen through the lives of Freud, Einstein, Picasso, Stravinsky, Eliot, Graham, and Gandhi. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gros, F. (2014). A philosophy of walking. London: Verso Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry, G. T. (1990). Practical sampling. London: Sage Publications.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Castelli, D. M., Hall, E. E., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience, 159(3), 1044–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaacson, W. (2011). Steve Jobs. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jin, P. (1992). Efficacy of Tai Chi, brisk walking, meditation, and reading in reducing mental and emotional stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 36(4), 361–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahnemann, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29(2), 169–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labonté-LeMoyne, É., Santhanam, R., Léger, P. M., Courtemanche, F., Fredette, M., & Sénécal, S. (2015). The delayed effect of treadmill desk usage on recall and attention. Computers in Human Behavior,. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.054.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, R. E., Goldberg, J. H., Sallis, J. F., Hickmann, S. A., Castro, C. M., & Chen, A. H. (2001). A prospective analysis of the relationship between walking and mood in sedentary ethnic minority women. Women and Health, 32(4), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, J. A. (2010). Health-chair reform your chair: Comfortable but deadly. Diabetes, 59(11), 2715–2716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Max, D.T. (2013). Two hit wonder. New Yorker. http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2013/10/21/two-hit-wonder. Accessed 12 Dec 2014.

  • Maxwell, J. A. (1996). Qualitative research design: An interactive approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morita, E., Fukuda, S., Nagano, J., Hamajima, N., Yamamoto, H., Iwai, Y., et al. (2007). Psychological effects of forest environments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing, walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health, 121(1), 54–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, R. (2013). ‘It’s not a hobby’: Reconceptualizing the place of writing in academic work. Higher Education, 66(1), 79–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nietzsche, F. W. (2007). Twilight of the Idols with Antichrist and Ecce Homo. Hertfordshire, UK: Wordsworth Editions Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(37), 15583–15587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppezzo, M., & Schwarz, D. L. (2014). Give your ideas some legs: The positive effect of walking on creative thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(4), 1142–1152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, N., Bauman, A., & Brown, W. (2009). Too much sitting: A novel and important predictor of chronic disease risk? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 43(2), 81–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagnoni, G. (2012). Dynamical properties of BOLD activity from the ventral posteromedial cortex associated with meditation and attentional skills. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(15), 5242–5249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R., & Rizzo, M. (2007). Neuroergonomics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel, A. V., Bernstein, L., Deka, A., Feigleson, H. S., Campbell, P. T., Gapstur, S. M., et al. (2010). Leisure time spent sitting in relation to total mortality in a prospective cohort of US adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 172(4), 419–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ralston, H. J. (1976). Energetics of human walking. In R. M. Herman (Ed.), Neural control of locomotion (pp. 77–98). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rebok, G. W., & Balcerak, L. J. (1989). Memory self-efficacy and performance differences in young and old adults: The effect of mnemonic training. Developmental Psychology, 25(5), 714–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossman, G. B., & Rallis, S. F. (1998). Learning in the field: An introduction to qualitative research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sakuragi, S., & Sugiyama, Y. (2006). Effects of daily walking on subjective symptoms, mood and autonomic nervous function. Journal of physiological anthropology., 25(4), 281–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, P. (2001). Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: A unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(1), 33–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaefer, S., Lövdén, M., Wieckhorst, B., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Cognitive performance is improved while walking: Differences in cognitive–sensorimotor couplings between children and young adults. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(3), 371–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, S. (2003). Walking as spiritual practice: The pilgrimage to Santiago de Compostela. Body and Society, 9(3), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solnit, R. (2001). Wanderlust: A history of walking. New York, NY: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: A neurobiological hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 31(7), 977–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). The basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veerman, L. J., Healy, G. N., Cobiac, L. J., Vos, T., Winkler, E. A. H., Owen, N., & Dunstan, D. W. (2011). Television viewing time and reduced life expectancy: A life table analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 46, 927–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, R. (1994). Learning from strangers: The art and method of qualitative interview. New York, NY: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weuve, J., Kang, J. H., Manson, J. E., Breteler, M. M., Ware, J. H., & Grodstein, F. (2004). Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA, 292(12), 1454–1461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilmot, E. G., Edwardson, C. L., Achana, F. A., Davies, M. J., Gorely, T., Gray, L. J., et al. (2012). Sedentary time in adults and the association with diabetes, cardiovascular disease and death: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Diabetologia, 55(11), 2895–2905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 625–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winerman, L. (2012). A machine for jumping to conclusions. Monitor on Psychology. 43, 2. Retrieved online at http://www.apa.org/monitor/2012/02/conclusions.aspx. Accessed 12 Nov 2014.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mia Keinänen.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Keinänen, M. Taking your mind for a walk: a qualitative investigation of walking and thinking among nine Norwegian academics. High Educ 71, 593–605 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10734-015-9926-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10734-015-9926-2

Keywords

Navigation