Skip to main content
Log in

Is Subjective Ill-Being Related to Islamophobia in Germany? In Search for Moderators

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Is subjective ill-being, defined as the inverse of subjective well-being, related to Islamophobia in Germany? We conducted a study guided by two goals to answer this question. The first goal was to test the hypothesis that subjective ill-being is associated with Islamophobia. The second goal, contingent on the results of testing for the association between subjective ill-being and Islamophobia, was to test a set of variables presumed to moderate this relationship—positive and negative contact with Muslims, right-wing political views, political participation, the importance of political life, and cultural diversity orientation. Data from the GESIS Panel, a probability-based panel representative of the German-speaking population aged between 18 and 70 years permanently residing in Germany, were used to test the study hypotheses. The data provided support for the hypothesis that subjective ill-being is indeed associated with Islamophobia in Germany (r = .12, p < .01). The data provided support for only one of the moderator hypothesis, namely cultural diversity orientation. Specifically, the data showed that the relationship between subjective ill-being and Islamophobia is stronger for those who have low (rather than high) cultural diversity orientation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Ed Diener, the father of the subjective well-being movement, defines subjective well-being as a broad category of phenomena that includes a preponderance of positive over negative emotions, satisfaction with life domains (satisfaction in work life, social life, family life, etc.), and global judgments of life satisfaction (Diener and Tov 2012).

  2. Subjective ill-being in our study is treated as the inverse of subjective well-being. That is, subjective ill-being is the other polar end of subjective well-being. Although there is debate in the subjective well-being literature about the distinction between subjective well-being and ill-being as being two distinct factors (e.g., Headey et al. 1984), we agree with those scholars who argue that the distinction between mental well-being and ill-being is counterproductive (e.g., Wood and Joseph 2010). In essence, subjective well-being should not be treated as a construct independent from subjective ill-being. A continuum approach should be used in conceptualizing both subjective well-being and ill-being, with positive functioning as one polar end of the continuum and negative functioning at the other end.

  3. Sections dbaw, dbbd, dcbi, dczy, dbbd, and dcbi according to the codebook of the GESIS Panel, which can be retrieved via www.gesis-panel.org.

  4. German citizenship does not reflect religious faith. The GESIS Panel does not include survey items related to religious faith because, under German law, such data require special data protection.

  5. With regard to categorical variables, t test and ANOVA tests were conducted to identify the relationships between categorical and continuous variables.

References

  • Allen, C. (2010). Islamophobia. London: Ashgate Publishing Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, C., & Nielsen, J. S. (2002). Summary report on Islamophobia in the EU after 11 September 2001. Vienna: European Monitoring Centre on Racism and Xenophobia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, J. B. (2018). The psychology of happiness in the modern world: A social psychological approach. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amnesty International. (2012). Choice and prejudice: Discrimination against muslims in Europe. London: Amnesty International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ata, A., Bastian, B., & Lusher, D. (2009). Intergroup contact in context: The mediating role of social norms and group-based perceptions on the contact-prejudice link. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,33, 498–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauder, H., & Semmelroggen, J. (2009). Immigration and imagination of nationhood in the German parliament. Nationalism and Ethnic Politics,15, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, S., Ter Wal, J., Lipinski, A., et al. (2011). Thematic Report 2011/02: Media content. MEDIVA: Media for diversity and migrant Integration. San Domenico di Fiesole, Italy: European University Institute, Badia Fiesolana. Available at: http://cadmus.eui.eu/bitstream/handle/1814/19730/MEDIVA_THEMATIC_REPORT_rev.pdf?sequence=1. Accessed 18 Sept 2013.

  • Berkowitz, L. (1969). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin,106, 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bierly, M. M. (1985). Prejudice toward contemporary outgroups as a generalized attitude. Journal of Applied Sociology,15, 189–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilefsky, D., & Homola, V. (2017). Austrian parliament bans full facial veils in public. The New York Times, May 17, 2017. Available at: https://www.nytimes.com/2017/05/17/world/europe/austria-veil-ban-muslim.html. Accessed 29 Oct 2017.

  • Bleich, E. (2009). Where do muslims stand on ethno-racial hierarchies in Britain and France: Evidence from public opinion surveys, 1988–2008. Patterns of Prejudice,43, 379–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosnjak, M., Dannwolf, T., Enderle, T., Schaurer, I., Struminskaya, B., Tanner, A., et al. (2018). Establishing an open probability-based mixed-mode panel of the general population in Germany: The GESIS panel. Social Science Computer Review,36, 103–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crisp, R. J., & Turner, R. N. (2011). Cognitive adaptation to the experience of social and cultural diversity. Psychological Bulletin,137, 242–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowson, M. (2009). Right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation: as mediators of worldview beliefs on attitudes related to the war on terror. Social Psychology,40, 93–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. A., Eckersley, R., Pallant, J., Van Vugt, J., & Misajon, R. (2003). Developing a national index of subjective wellbeing: The Australian Unity Wellbeing Index. Social Indicators Research,64, 159–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Tov, W. (2012). National accounts of well-being. In K. C. Land, A. C. Michalos, & M. J. Sirgy (Eds.), Handbook of social indicators and quality of life research (pp. 137–157). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ely, R. J., & Thomas, D. A. (2001). Cultural diversity at work: The effects of diversity perspectives on work group processes and outcomes. Administrative Science Quarterly,46, 229–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esposito, J. L., & Kalin, I. (Eds.). (2011). Islamophobia: The challenge of pluralism in the 21st century. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Euractiv. (2015). Poll: Majority of Germans reject immigration from outside the EU. Available at: https://www.euractiv.com/section/justice-home-affairs/news/poll-majority-of-germans-reject-immigration-from-outside-the-eu/. Accessed 13 Aug 2018.

  • Factank. (2018). Differing views of immigrants pose a test for Germany’s coalition government. Available at: http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/06/21/differing-views-of-immigrants-pose-a-test-for-germanys-coalition-government/. Accessed 13 Aug 2018.

  • Gallup World. (2013). Islamophobia: understanding anti-Muslim sentiment in the West. Available at: http://www.gallup.com/poll/157082/islamophobia-understanding-anti-muslim-sentimentwest.aspx. Accessed 10 Jan 2013.

  • Garofalo, J. (1981). The fear of crime: Causes and consequences. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology,72, 839–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gay, G. (2010). Acting on beliefs in teacher education for cultural diversity. Journal of Teacher Education,61, 143–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression based approach. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B., Holmström, E., & Wearing, A. (1984). Well-being and ill-being: Different dimensions? Social Indicators Research,14, 115–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. (2004). Toward national well-being accounts. The American Economic Review,94, 429–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kepel, G., & Jardin, A. (2017). Terror in France: The rise of Jihad in the West. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kinder, D. R., & Kiewiet, D. R. (1979). Economic grievances and political behavior: The role of personal discontents and collective judgments in congressional voting. American Journal of Political Science,23, 495–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, A. L., Cummins, R. A., & McPherson, W. (2005). An investigation into the cross-cultural equivalence of the personal wellbeing index. Social Indicators Research,72, 403–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubbers, M., Gijsberts, M., & Scheepers, P. (2002). Extreme right-wing voting in Western Europe. European Journal of Political Research,41, 345–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClaren, L. (2003). Anti-immigrant prejudice in Europe: Contact, threat perception and preferences for the exclusion of immigrants. Social Forces,81, 909–936.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, K., Mason, P., & Lewis, J. (2008). Images of Islam in the UK. The representation of British Muslims in the national print news media 20002008. UK: Cardiff School of Journalism, Media and Cultural Studies. Available at: http://www.cardiff.ac.uk/jomec/resources/08channel4-dispatches.pdf. Accessed 17 Sept 2013.

  • Ogan, C., Willnat, L., Pennington, R., & Bashir, M. (2014). The rise of anti-muslim prejudice: Media and Islamophobia in Europe and the United States. The International Communication Gazette,76, 27–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parekh, B. (2001). Rethinking multiculturalism: Cultural diversity and political theory. Ethnicities,1, 109–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmela, M., & von Scheve, C. (2017). Emotional roots of right-wing political populism. Social Science Information,56, 567–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sirgy, M. J. (2012). The psychology of quality of life: Hedonic well-being, life satisfaction, and Eudaimonia. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sputnik International. (2016). New poll finds Germans want immediate freeze on immigration. Available at: https://sputniknews.com/europe/201611111047337512-germans-immigration-freeze-poll/. Accessed 13 Aug 2018.

  • Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues,41, 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorisdottir, H., Jost, J. T., Liviatan, I., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). Psychological needs and values underlying left-right political orientation: Cross-national evidence from Eastern and Western Europe. Public Opinion Quarterly,71, 175–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uenal, F. (2016). Disentangling Islamophobia: The differential effects of symbolic, realistic, and terroristic threat perceptions as mediators between social dominance orientation and Islamophobia. Journal of Social and Political Psychology,4, 66–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wike, R., Stokes, B., & Simmons, K. (2016). Europeans fear wave of refugees will mean more terrorism, fewer jobs. Pew Research Center,11, 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wikipedia. (2018). Demographics of Germany. Available at: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Demographics_of_Germany. Accessed 25 Nov 2018.

  • Wodak, R. (2015). The politics of fear: What right-wing populist discourses mean. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, A. M., & Joseph, S. (2010). The absence of positive psychological (eudemonic) well-being as a risk factor for depression: A ten year cohort study. Journal of Affective Disorders,122, 213–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zick, A., Kupper, B., & Hovermann, A. (2011). Intolerance, prejudice and discrimination: A European report. Berlin: Friederich Ebert Stiftung.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to M. Joseph Sirgy.

Appendix

Appendix

Results of regression and moderation analyses (outcome: islamophobia)

Hypothesis

Predictors

Coefficient

SE

t

p

95% CI

Low

High

H2

Constant

.350

.317

1.102

.271

− .273

.973

 

Subj. ill-being

.079

.025

3.188

.001

.030

.128

 

Positive contact

− .307

.028

− 10.878

.000

− .363

− .252

 

Subj. Ill-being × positive contact

.032

.025

1.303

.193

− .016

.081

 

Age

− .002

.002

− 1.158

.247

− .007

.002

 

Marital status

.011

.030

.350

.726

− .049

.070

 

Gender

− .240

.052

− 4.578

.000

− .342

− .137

 

Education

− .062

.013

− 4.737

.000

− .088

− .036

 

Citizenship status

.103

.085

1.216

.224

− .063

.269

 

Living standard

.161

.041

3.880

.000

.080

.242

 

Social welfare

.026

.119

.224

.823

− .206

.259

 

R2 = .232, F = 24.647, df = 10, 814, p = .0000

H3

Constant

− .111

.329

− .336

.737

− .757

.536

 

Subj. ill-being

.056

.026

2.188

.029

.006

.107

 

negative contact

.277

.043

6.378

.000

.192

.362

 

Subj. Ill-being × negative contact

.051

.032

1.586

.113

− .012

.114

 

Age

.003

.002

1.282

.200

− .002

.007

 

Marital status

.006

.031

.201

.841

− .055

.068

 

gender

− .171

.055

− 3.137

.002

− .278

− .064

 

Education

− .070

.014

− 5.142

.000

− .096

− .043

 

Citizenship status

.011

.088

.129

.897

− .161

.184

 

Living standard

.172

.043

4.017

.000

.088

.256

 

Social welfare

.127

.124

1.031

.303

− .115

.370

 

R2 = .170, F = 16.598, df = 10, 813, p = .0000

H4

Constant

.438

.189

2.318

.021

.068

.809

 

Subj. ill-being

.079

.015

5.335

.000

.050

.108

 

Right-wing view

.598

.022

27.446

.000

.555

.641

 

Subj. Ill-being × right-wing view

− .020

.018

− 1.134

.257

− .056

.015

 

Age

− .005

.001

− 3.839

.000

− .007

− .002

 

Marital status

.020

.018

1.156

.248

− .014

.055

 

Gender

− .132

.030

− 4.327

.000

− .191

− .072

 

Education

− .028

.008

− 3.548

.000

− .043

− .012

 

Citizenship status

.130

.052

2.507

.012

.028

.231

 

Living standard

.050

.024

2.029

.043

.002

.098

 

Social welfare

− .058

.069

− .852

.394

− .193

.076

 

R2 = .248, F = 99.019, df = 10, 2996, p = .0000

H5

Constant

.312

.205

1.519

.129

− .091

.714

 

Subj. ill-being

.071

.016

4.356

.000

.039

.103

 

Politically activeness

− .294

.047

− 6.290

.000

− .385

− .202

 

Subj. Ill-being × politically activeness

.053

.033

1.628

.104

− .011

.117

 

Age

− .001

.001

− .851

.395

− .004

.002

 

Marital status

.025

.019

1.280

.201

− .013

.063

 

Gender

− .139

.033

− 4.171

.000

− .204

− .074

 

Education

− .069

.008

− 8.208

.000

− .085

− .052

 

Citizenship status

.131

.053

2.452

.014

.026

.236

 

Living standard

.101

.026

3.812

.000

.049

.152

 

Social welfare

− .024

.074

− .326

.745

− .170

.121

 

R2 = .069, F = 23.109, df = 10, 3131, p = .0000

H6

Constant

.271

.207

1.305

.192

− .136

.677

 

Subj. ill-being

.070

.016

4.260

.000

.038

.102

 

Importance on political life

− .122

.024

− 5.001

.000

− .170

− .074

 

Subj. Ill-being × importance on political life

− .034

.020

− 1.756

.079

− .073

.004

 

Age

.000

.001

.237

.813

− .002

.003

 

Marital status

.022

.019

1.153

.249

− .016

.061

 

Gender

− .164

.034

− 4.856

.000

− .231

− .098

 

Education

− .069

.008

− 8.126

.000

− .085

− .052

 

Citizenship status

.139

.054

2.576

.010

.033

.244

 

Living standard

.094

.027

3.534

.000

.042

.147

 

Social welfare

− .019

.075

− .256

.798

− .166

.128

 

R2 = .068, F = 22.488, df = 10, 3014, p = .0000

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sirgy, M.J., Kim, M.Y., Joshanloo, M. et al. Is Subjective Ill-Being Related to Islamophobia in Germany? In Search for Moderators. J Happiness Stud 20, 2655–2675 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0063-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0063-3

Keywords

Navigation