Skip to main content
Log in

Assessing implicit motives with an online version of the picture story exercise

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

An online version of a Picture Story Exercise (PSE) was administered to 180 participants to assess implicit motives for achievement, affiliation, and power. First, data were analyzed with respect to different aspects of participant behavior (dropout, writing time, story length, setting). Second, online data were compared with data collected in the laboratory (n = 93). Results regarding motive density, internal consistency, picture profiles, and position effects were comparable among samples and consistent with previous research (Pang and Schultheiss in J Person Assess 85:280–294, 2005; Schultheiss and Brunstein in J Person Assess 77: 71–86, 2001). In addition, online participants completed self-report measures assessing explicit motives and the Big Five personality factors. Explicit motives showed no substantial overlap with PSE motive scores, replicating previous research. Personality factors revealed gender differences in correlates: men with a high implicit power motive reported less agreeableness and conscientiousness. Results suggest that administering the PSE online yields reliable and valid data.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For a recent list of studies conducted online see the American Psychological Society (APS) experiment list maintained by John Krantz: http://psych.hanover.edu/research/exponnet.html.

  2. Typed stories were found to be by a third longer than handwritten ones, but when corrected for word count the two methods of administration produce comparable motive scores (Blankenship and Zoota 1998; Schultheiss et al. 2008a).

  3. The PSE assessment was part of a questionnaire for a larger research project on long-term relationships.

  4. Six participants did not provide stories to all four picture cues and were therefore not included in the analysis for overall word count.

  5. When we used raw motive scores we found the same effects: both the Picture × Motive interaction, F(2, 215) = 70.28, p < 0.001, n² = 0.25, and the three-way interaction were significant, F(6, 211) = 2.34, p < 0.05. n² = 0.01.

References

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1981). Studying personality in the context of an advanced motivational psychology. American Psychologist, 36, 117–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: Stress-dependent discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 781–799.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Birnbaum, M. H. (2001). Introduction to behavioral research on the Internet. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birnbaum, M. H. (2004a). Methodological and ethical issues in conducting social psychology research via the Internet. In C. Sansone, C. C. Morf, & A. T. Panter (Eds.), Handbook of methods in social psychology (pp. 359–382). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birnbaum, M. H. (2004b). Human research and data collection via the internet. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 803–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blankenship, V., & Zoota, A. L. (1998). Comparing power imagery in TATs written by hand or on the computer. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30, 441–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. (1985). A longitudinal study of ego and cognitive development. Research grant application. Berkeley: University of California Department of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., & Hoyer, S. (2002). Implizites und explizites Leistungsstreben: Befunde zur Unabhängigkeit zweier Motivationssysteme [Implicit versus explicit achievement strivings: Empirical evidence of the independence of two motivational systems]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 16, 51–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: Two separate but interacting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 205–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, T. (2000). Potential of the Internet for personality research. In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Psychological experiments on the Internet (pp. 121–140). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, T. (2001). Online personality assessment. In U.-D. Reips & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Dimensions of internet science (pp. 57–74). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, T., Johnson, J. A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2005). Implementing a five- personality inventory for use on the internet. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, 115–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, T., & Smith, J. L. (1999). Using the internet for psychological research: Personality testing on the world-wide web. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 125–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • deCharms, R., Morrison, H., Reitman, W., & McClelland, D. (1955). Behavioral correlates of directly measured and indirectly measured achievement motivation. In D. McClelland (Ed.), Studies in motivation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillman, D. A., & Bowker, D. K. (2001). The Web questionnaire challenge to survey methodologists. In U.-D. Reips & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Dimensions of internet science (pp. 159–178). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1997). Sex differences in social behavior: Comparing social role theory and evolutionary psychology. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1380–1383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A., & McAdams, D. P. (1991). Personal strivings and motive dispositions: Exploring the links. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(6), 648–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falbo, T. (1977). Relationships between sex, sex role, and social influence. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 2(1), 62–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, A., Bächtiger, M. T., & Reips, U.-D. (2001). Financial incentives, personal information, and drop-outs in online studies. In U.-D. Reips & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Dimensions of internet science (pp. 209–219). Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59, 93–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., & Brandstätter, V. (2009). To get a taste of your goals: Creating motive-goal congruence by affect-focus goal-fantasy. Journal of Personality, 77, 1527–1560.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., Oertig, D., Allemand, M., & Brandstätter, V. (in press). Motive-discrepancies as antecedents of unhealthy eating behavior. Journal of Personality.

  • Kehr, H. M. (2004). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 315–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • King, L. A. (1995). Wishes, motives, goals, and personal memories: Relations of measures of human motivation. Journal of Personality, 63, 985–1007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1968). Short-term stability and concurrent validity of TAT and need scores: Achievement, affiliation and hostile press. In Proceedings of the 76th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, American Psychologist, 23, 878.

  • Langan-Fox, J., & Grant, S. (2006). The thematic apperception test: Towards a standard measure of the big three motives. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 277–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundy, A. C. (1980). Conditions of test administration and TAT validity. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, Department of Psychology and Social Relations.

  • Lundy, A. C. (1981). Situationally evoked schema sets in TAT-based research: A cognitive explanation for differential TAT validity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.

  • Lundy, A. C. (1985). The reliability of the thematic apperception test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 141–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1991). The personal value questionnaire. Boston: McBer & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., & Jemmott, J. B. (1980). Power motivation, stress and physical illness. Journal of Human Stress, 6, 6–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., & Pilon, D. A. (1983). Sources of adult motives in patterns of parent behavior in early childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic apperception test manual. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musch, J., & Klauer, K. C. (2002). Psychological experimenting on the World Wide Web: Investigating content effects in syllogistic reasoning. In B. Batinic, U.-D. Reips, & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Online social sciences (pp. 181–212). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in U.S. college students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 280–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 203–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reips, U.-D. (2000). The web experiment method: Advantages, disadvantages, and solutions. In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Psychological experiments on the Internet (pp. 89–117). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Reips, U.-D. (2001). The web experimental psychology lab: Five years of data collection on the internet. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 33, 201–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reips, U.-D. (2002a). Standards for internet experimenting. Experimental Psychology, 49(4), 243–256.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reips, U.-D. (2002b). Internet-based psychological experimenting: Five dos and five don’ts. Social Science Computer Review, 20(3), 241–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reips, U.-D. (2002c). Context effects in web surveys. In B. Batinic, U.-D. Reips, & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Online social sciences. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reips, U.-D., & Bosnjak, M. (2001). Dimensions of internet science. Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reips, U.-D., Morger, V., & Meier B. (2001). Fünfe gerade sein lassen: Listenkontexteffekte beim Kategorisieren [“Letting five be equal”: List context effects in categorization]. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Reips, U.-D., & Neuhaus, C. (2002). WEXTOR: A Webbased tool for generating and visualizing experimental designs and procedures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 34, 234–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuman, D. A. (1982). Ipsative behavioral variability and the quality of thematic apperceptive measurement of the achievement motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(5), 1098–1110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Implicit motives. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 603–633). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (Eds.). (2010). Implicit motives. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Liening, S., & Schad, D. (2008a). The reliability of a picture story exercise measure of implicit motives: Estimates of internal consistency, retest reliability, and ipsative stability. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1560–1571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Pang, J. S. (2007). Measuring implicit motives. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 322–344). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., Waugh, C. E., Stanton, S. J., Meier, E., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2008b). Exploring the motivational brain: Effects of implicit power motivation on brain activation in response to facial expressions of emotion. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 333–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Yankova, D., Dirlikov, B., & Schad, D. (2009). Are implicit and explicit motive measures statistically independent? A fair and balanced test using the picture story exercise and a cue- and response-matched questionnaire measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 72–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, S., & Reips, U.-D. (2001). CGI versus JavaScript: A web experiment on the reversed hindsight bias. In U.-D. Reips & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Dimensions of internet science (pp. 75–90). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. P. (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. P., Feld, S. C., & Franz, C. E. (1992). Methodological considerations: Steps in research employing content analysis systems. In C. P. Smith (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 515–536). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, S. J., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2009). The hormonal correlates of implicit power motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 942–949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thrash, T. M., Elliot, A. J., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). Methodological and dispositional predictors of congruence between implicit and explicit need for achievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 961–974.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1988). The power motive in women and men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 510–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1991). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.

  • Winter, D. G. (1992). Power motivation revisited. In C. P. Smith (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 311–324). Cambridge: University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (4th edn). Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

  • Winter, D. G. (2010). Power in the person: Exploring the psychological underground of power. In A. Guinote & T. Vescio (Eds.), The social psychology of power. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105, 230–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1977). Power motive reliability as a function of test-retest instructions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 436–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. (1978). The power motive. In H. London & J. Exner (Eds.), Dimensions of personality (pp. 391–447). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woike, B., Lavezzary, E., & Barsky, J. (2001). The influence of implicit motives on memory processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 935–945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woike, B., McLeod, S., & Goggin, M. (2003). Implicit and explicit motives influence accessibility to different autobiographical knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1046–1055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowlegments

This research was supported in part by the Studienstiftung des deutschen Volkes with a scholarship awarded to the first author. We thank Alena Friedrich, Yousuf Haq, and Rebecca Wheeler, for their help in collecting and coding the data, Carol Dweck and Ulf Reips for their support in conducting this research, and Gregory Walton and David Yeager for helpful comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katharina Bernecker.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bernecker, K., Job, V. Assessing implicit motives with an online version of the picture story exercise. Motiv Emot 35, 251–266 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9175-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9175-8

Keywords

Navigation