Skip to main content
Log in

Context-sensitive rules and word naming in Italian children

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study examines the role of orthographic complexity on Italian children’s word reading. Two experiments are reported in which elementary school children (3rd and 5th graders) read aloud words containing simple or contextual letter-sound conversion rules. In Experiment 1, both groups of participants read words containing contextual rules more slowly and less accurately than words containing simple, non-contextual rules. Experiment 2 showed that the effect of rule complexity held for low but not high frequency words, on both reading speed and accuracy. No interactions with grade were found. This pattern is in line with previous findings on the effects of rule contextuality on adult performance [Burani, C. Barca, L. & Ellis, A. W. (2006). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 346–352]. Despite the regularity of the Italian orthography, the presence of complex rules influences both reading speed and accuracy of young readers. Moreover, the reading system of readers of a shallow orthography seems efficient from the first years of reading instruction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andrews, S. (1982). Phonological recoding: Is the regularity effect consistent? Memory & Cognition, 10, 565–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 34, 424–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Burani, C., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Italian developmental dyslexic and proficient readers: Where are the differences? Brain and Language, 98, 347–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A., & Deavers, R. P. (1999). Units of analysis in nonword reading: Evidence from children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 208–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burani, C., Barca, L., & Ellis, A. W. (2006). Orthographic complexity and word naming in Italian: Some words are more transparent than others. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 346–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burani, C., Marcolini, S., & Stella, G. (2002). How early does morpho-lexical reading develop in readers of a shallow orthography? Brain and Language, 81, 568–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, V., & Leahy, J. (1992). Children’s and adults’ reading of nonwords: Effects of regularity and consistency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 718–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., & Gugliotta, M. (1995). Visual and phonological determinants of misreading in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 7, 237–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. C., Natsume, M., Stavropoulou, K., Hoxhallari, L., van Daal, V. H. P., Polyzoe, N., Tsipa, M., & Petalas, M. (2004). The effects of orthographic depth on learning to read alphabetic, syllabic, and logographic scripts. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 438–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount: Implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 777–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & De Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. (2001). Are there orthographic impairments in phonological dyslexia? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 71–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K. (2000). Influences of orthographic consistency and reading instruction on the development of nonword reading skills. European Journal of Psychology and Education, 15, 239–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepschy, A. L., & Lepschy, G. C. (1991). The Italian language today. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masterson, J., Laxon, V., & Stuart, M. (1992). Beginning reading with phonology. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayall, K. (2002). Case-mixing effects on children’s word recognition: Lexical feedback and development. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 525–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzotta S., Barca, L., Marcolini, S., Stella, G., & Burani, C. (2005). Frequenza, immaginabilità ed età di acquisizione delle parole: in che misura queste variabili influenzano la lettura dei bambini italiani? [How do frequency, imageability and word age-of-acquisition affect Italian children’s reading aloud?]. Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 9, 249–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monaghan, J., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). What, exactly, interacts with spelling-sound consistency in word naming? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, Cognition, 28, 183–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Tosi, V., De Nigris, & Carrieri, R. (2005). From phonological recoding to lexical reading: A longitudinal study on reading development In Italian. Language and Cognitive Processes (available online).

  • Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cotelli, M., Cossu, G., Corte, F., Lorusso, M., Pesenti, S., Gallagher, A., Perani, D., Price, C., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 91–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). A further look at the “Language-as-fixed-effect fallacy”. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 141–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1998). Whammies and double whammies: The length effect on nonword reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 277–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rey, A., & Schiller, N. O. (2005). Graphemic complexity and multiple print-to-sound associations in visual word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 33, 76–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (1985). Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e disortografia evolutiva [Battery for the diagnosis of developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia]. Firenze: Organization Special.

  • Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 383–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorstad, G. (1991). The effect of orthography on the acquisition of literacy skills. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 527–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tressoldi, P. E. (1996). L’evoluzione della lettura e della scrittura dalla II elementare alla III media [The development of reading and writing from grade 2 to year 8]. Età Evolutiva, 53, 43–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waters, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & Bruck, M. (1984). Children’s and adults’ use of spelling-sound information in three reading tasks. Memory & Cognition, 12, 293–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. C. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Simone Mazzotta for help with collecting the data. This work was supported by MIUR-FIRB Grant RBAU01LE9P to Cristina Burani. The authors are members of the Marie Curie Research and Training Network: Language and Brain (RTN:LAB) funded by the European Commission (MRTN-CT-2004-512141) as part of its Sixth Framework Programme.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura Barca.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barca, L., Ellis, A.W. & Burani, C. Context-sensitive rules and word naming in Italian children. Read Writ 20, 495–509 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9040-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9040-z

Keywords

Navigation