Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Why neuroscience matters to cognitive neuropsychology

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The broad issue in this paper is the relationship between cognitive psychology and neuroscience. That issue arises particularly sharply for cognitive neurospsychology, some of whose practitioners claim a methodological autonomy for their discipline. They hold that behavioural data from neuropsychological impairments are sufficient to justify assumptions about the underlying modular structure of human cognitive architecture, as well as to make inferences about its various components. But this claim to methodological autonomy can be challenged on both philosophical and empirical grounds. A priori considerations about (cognitive) multiple realisability challenge the thesis on philosophical grounds, and neuroscientific findings from developmental disorders substantiate that challenge empirically. The conclusion is that behavioural evidence alone is inadequate for scientific progress since appearances of modularity can be thoroughly deceptive, obscuring both the dynamic processes of neural development and the endstate network architecture of real cognitive systems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs R., Sears L., Piven J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13(2): 232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30: 285–298

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S. (1998). Modularity in developmental cognitive neuropsychology: Evidence from autism and Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. In Burack J. (ed). Handbook of mental retardation and development. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 334–348

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S.W.S.J.T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition 4: 311

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi U., Birhle A., Jernigan T., Trauner D., Doherty S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics 6: 115–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Lechtenberger, W., Jones, W., Lai, Z., St. George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. In U. Bellugi & M. St. George (Eds.), Journey from cognition to brain to gene: Perspectives from Williams syndrome. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Bellugi U., Lichtenberger L., Mills D., Galaburda A., Korenberg J.R. (1999). Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Trends in Neurosciences 22, 197–207

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Wang P. P., & Jernigan, T. (1994). Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsychological profile. In S. Broman & J. Grafman (Eds.), Atypcial cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: Implications for brain function. Erlbaum.

  • Bishop D.V.M. (1997). Cognitive neuropsychology and developmental disorders: uncomfortable bedfellows. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 50A(4): 899–923

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1995). The mind as the software of the brain. In Smith, E., Osherson, D. (eds). Thinking: an invitation to cognitive science. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.

  • Boucher J., Lewis V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33(5): 843–859

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher J., Lewis V., Collis G. (2000). Familiar face and voice matching and recognition in children with autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 39(02): 171–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza A. (1986). On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from an analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies. Brain and Cognition 5: 41–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza A. (1992). Is cognitive neuropsychology possible?. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4(1): 80–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza A., Coltheart M. (2006). Cognitive neuropsychology twenty years on. Cognitive Neuropsychology 23(1): 3–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey S., Diamond R. (1977). From piecemeal to configural representation of faces. Science 195, 312–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung M.-S., Thompson D.M. (1995). Development of face recognition. British Journal of Psychology 86(1): 55

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland P.M., Churchland P.S. (1996). Replies from the Churchlands. In: McCauley R.N. (ed). The Churchlands and their critics. Oxford, Blackwell Publishers.

  • Coltheart M. (2002a). Cognitive neuropsychology. In Wixted J. (ed). Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology (vol. 4). New York, Wiley & Sons, pp. 139–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (2002b). The distinction between proximal and distal causes of developmental disorders of cognition. Presented in the Seminar Series, Research School of Social Sciences, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia.

  • Coltheart M. (2004). Brain imaging, connectionism, and cognitive neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology 21(1): 21–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart M., Curtis B., Atkins P., Haller M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual route and parallel-distributed processing approaches. Psychological Review 100: 589–608

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart M., Jackson N.E. (1998). Defining dyslexia. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review 3(01): 12–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart M., Langdon R. (1998). Autism, modularity and levels of explanation in cognitive science. Mind and Language 13(1): 138–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton K., Nacewicz B., Johnstone T., Schaefer H., Gernsbacher M.A., Goldsmith H.H., Alexander A., Davidson R. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience 8(4): 519–526

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies S., Bishop D., Manstead A.S.R., Tantam D. (1994). Face perception in children with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35(6): 1033–1057

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson G., Carver L., Meltzoff A.N., Panagiotides H., McPartland J., Webb S.J. (2002). Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Development 73(3): 700–717

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Hahn M. (2001). The neuropsychology of face processing during infancy and childhood. In: Nelson C.A., Luciana M. (eds). Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, pp. 381–398

    Google Scholar 

  • de Schonen S., Mathivet E., Deurelle C. (1989). A timing puzzle. Current Psychology of Cognition 9: 147–161

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Deruelle C., Mancini J., Livet M.O., Casse-Perrot C., de Schonen S. (1999). Configural and local processing of faces in children with Williams Syndrome. Brain and Cognition 41(3): 276–298

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elgar K., Campbell R. (2001). Annotation: The cognitive neuroscience of face recognition: Implications for developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42(6): 705–717

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis H.D., Ellis D.M., Fraser W., Deb S. (1994). A preliminary study of right hemisphere cognitive deficits and impaired social judgments among young people with Asperger syndrome. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 3(4): 255–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith U., Happé F. (1994). Autism: beyond ‘theory of mind’. Cognition 50: 115–132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier I., Logothetis N.K. (2000). Is face recognition not so special after all?. Cognitive Neuropsychology 17(1): 125–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerrans P. (2003a). Nativism and neuroconstructivism in the explanation of Williams syndrome. Biology and Philosophy 18(1): 41–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerrans P. (2003b). Nativism, neuroconstructivism, and developmental disorder. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(06): 757–758

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik M. (1997). Language deficits and genetic factors. Trends in Cognitive Science 1, 5–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice S., Spratling M.W., Karmiloff-Smith A., Halit H., Csibra G., De Haan M., Johnson M.H. (2001). Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Neuroreport 12(12): 2697–2700

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grill-Spector K., Knouf N., Kanwisher N. (2004). The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nature Neuroscience 7(5), 555–562

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hadjikhani N., Joseph R., Snyder J., Chabris C., Clark J., Steele S., McGarth L., Vangel M., Aharon I., Feczko E., Harris G., Tager-Flusberg H. (2004). Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. NeuroImage 22, 1141–1150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive style or cognitive deficit. Trends in Cognitive Science 3(6): 216–222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harley T.A. (2004a). Does cognitive neuropsychology have a future?. Cognitive Neuropsychology 21(1): 3–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harley T.A. (2004b). Promises, promises. Cognitive Neuropsychology 21(1): 51–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haxby J.V., Hoffman E.A., Gobbini M.I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 223–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson R.P., Ouston J., Lee A. (1988). Emotion recognition in autism: Coordinating faces and voices. Psychological Medicine 18, 911–923

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N. E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure: Toward an integrated cognitive psychology of atypical reading. Psychology Press.

  • Johnson M.H. (2000). Functional brain development in infants: Elements of an interactive specialization framework. Child Development 71(1): 75–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson M.H., Halit H., Grice S.J., Karmiloff-Smith A. (2002). Neuroimaging of typical and atypical development: A perspective from multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology 14, 521–536

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph R.M., Tanaka J. (2003). Holistic and part-based recognition in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines 44(4): 529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher N., McDermott J., Chun M.M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience 17(11): 4302–4311

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA, Bradford/MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith A. (1997). Crucial differences between developmental cognitive neuroscience and adult neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology 13: 513–524

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Science 2(10): 389–398

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith A., Brown J.H., Grice S., Paterson S. (2003). Dethroning the myth: Cognitive dissociations and innate modularity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology 23(1/2): 227–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith A., Thomas M., Annaz D., Humphreys K., Ewing S., Brace N., Duuren M., Pike G., Grice S., Campbell R. (2004). Exploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: The importance of building developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45(7): 1258–1274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution task. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 41(07): 831–846

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin A., Sparrow S.S., de Bildt A., Cicchetti D.V., Cohen D.J., Volkmar F.R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29(6): 499–508

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn S.M., Van Kleeck M. (1990). Broken brains and normal minds: Why humpty dumpty needs a skeleton. In Schwartz E. (ed). Computational neuroscience. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, pp. 390–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Laing E., Butterworth G., Ansari D., Gsodl M., Longhi E., Panagiotaki G., Paterson S., Karmiloff-Smith A. (2002). Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science 5(2): 233–246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos I. (1974). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programs. In Lakatos I., Musgrave A. (eds). Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 91–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. (1991). The theory of mind impairment in autism: Evidence for a modular Mechanism of development? In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

  • Mehler J., Morton J., Jusczyk P.W. (1984). On reducing language to biology. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1, 83–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mervis C.B., Bertrand J. (1997). Developmental relations between cognition and language: Evidence from Williams syndrome. In: Adamson L.B., Romski M.A. (eds.) Research on communication and language disorders: Contributions to theories of language development. New York, Brookes, pp. 75–106

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills D.L., Alvarez T.D., St. George M., Appelbaum L.G., Bellugi U., Neville H. (2000). Electrophysiological studies of face processing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1): 47–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton J., Johnson M.H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLEARN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review 98(2): 164–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver A., Johnson M.H., Karmiloff-Smith A., Pennington B. (2000). Deviations in the emergence of representations: A neuroconstructivist framework for analysing developmental disorders. Developmental Science 3(1): 1–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling J., Dawson G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videos. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24, 247–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Passarotti A.M., Paul B.M., Bussiere J.R., Buxton R.B., Wong E.C., Stiles J. (2003). The development of face and location processing: An fMRI study. Developmental Science 6(1): 100–117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce K., Muller R.A., Ambrose J., Allen G., Courchesne E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain 124(10): 2059–2073

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S. (1999). Words and rules. New York/London, Basic Books/Weidenfield & Nicholson

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut D.C. (1995). Double dissociation without modularity: Evidence from connectionist neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 17(2): 291–321

    Google Scholar 

  • Price C.J., Friston K.J. (2005). Functional ontologies for cognition: The systematic definition of structure and function. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22(3): 262–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quartz S.R. (2003). Innateness and the brain. Biology and Philosophy 18(1): 13–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rae C., Karmiloff-Smith A., Lee M.A., Dixon R.M., Grant J., Blamire A.M., Thompson C.H., Styles P., Radda G.K. (1998). Brain biochemistry in Williams syndrome: Evidence for a role of the cerebellum in cognition?. Neurology 51(1): 33–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz R.T., Gautier I., Klin A., Fulbright R.K., Anderson A.W., Volkmar F., Skudlarski P., Lacadie C., Cohen D.J., Gore J.C. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 331–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith N. (1999). Chomsky: Ideas and ideals. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone T., Davies M. (1999). Autonomous psychology and the moderate neuron doctrine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(05): 849–850

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham J., Baron-Cohen S., Charman T., Cox A., Baird G., Drew A., Rees L., Wheelwright S. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial, stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 39, 747–753

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarr M.J., Gauthier I. (2000). FFA: A flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise. Nature Neuroscience 3(8): 764–769

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple C.M. (1997). Developmental cognitive neuropsychology. Hove, UK, Psychology Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas M., Karmiloff-Smith A. (2002). Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage?. Implications from connectionist modelling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(06): 727–750

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas M., & Karmiloff-Smith A. (2003). Residual normality: Friend or foe?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(06): 772–780

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Udwin O., Yule W. (1991). A cognitive and behavioral phenotype in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 13, 232–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden G.C., Pennington B.F., Stone G.O. (2001). What do double dissociations prove?. Cognitive Science 25(1): 111–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang P.P., Doherty S., Rourke S.B., Bellugi U. (1995). Unique profile of visuoperceptual skills in a genetic syndrome. Brain and Cognition 29, 54–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Victoria McGeer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McGeer, V. Why neuroscience matters to cognitive neuropsychology. Synthese 159, 347–371 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-007-9234-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-007-9234-1

Keywords

Navigation