Skip to main content
Log in

Aesthetics and action: situations, emotional perception and the Kuleshov effect

  • S.I. : Gestalt Phenomenology and Embodied Cognitive Science
  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article focuses on situations and emotional perception. To this end, I start with the Kuleshov effect wherein identical shots of performers manifest different expressions when cut to different contexts. However, I conducted experiments with a twist, using Darth Vader and non-primates, and even here expressions varied with contexts. Building on historically and conceptually linked Gibsonian, Gestalt, phenomenological and pragmatic schools, along with consonant experimental work, I extrapolate these results to defend three interconnected points. First, I argue that while perceiving expression is sometimes about reading minds, it is more squarely about perceiving solicitations or closures for action. Second, I frame expressions as a subcategory of Gibson’s affordances. This includes those showing up through context, in turn suggesting Kuleshov-like scenarios are not mere perceptual tricks. Third, I maintain that situations—with or without other human beings—have action-motivating expressive qualities or what Gestalt theorists called physiognomic characters, following Werner. These likewise emerge through ecological relations, and are very much like affordances and in the world as much as them. With resemblance theories, my account agrees that we perceive expression in the world. However, it indicates a broad range of cases in which expression shows up as consequences of overall situations that solicit or close actions, as opposed to mere resemblance between entities and human expressions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Screen selection at https://youtu.be/xegCVU2tsDU.

  2. Screen selection at https://youtu.be/kRvvlRbHfqM.

  3. Screen selection at https://youtu.be/CBkViIc1-Oo.

References

  • Arnheim, R. (1971). Art and visual perception: A psychology of the creative eye. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., et al. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19, 724–732.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012a). Holistic person processing: Faces with bodies tell the whole story. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 20–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012b). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338(6111), 1225–1229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barratt, D., Rédei, A. C., Innes-Ker, Å., & van de Weijer, J. (2016). Does the Kuleshov Effect really exist? Revisiting a classic film experiment on facial expressions and emotional contexts. Perception, 45, 847–874.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F. (2014). The conceptual act theory: A précis. Emotion Review, 6, 292–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. M. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096.

    Google Scholar 

  • Böhme, G. (1993). Atmosphere as the fundamental concept of a new aesthetic. Thesis Eleven, 36, 113–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2003). The devaluation effect: Activating a need devalues unrelated objects. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 463–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1994). Social and emotional functions in facial expression and communication: The readout hypothesis. Biological Psychology, 38, 95–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calbi, M., Heimann, K., Barratt, D., Siri, F., Umiltà, M. A., & Gallese, V. (2017). How context influences our perception of emotional faces: A behavioral study on the Kuleshov effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, N. (1993). Toward a theory of point-of-view editing: Communication, emotion, and the movies. Poetics Today, 14, 123–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 205–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A., & Käufer, S. (2016). Pragmatism, phenomenology, and extended cognition. In R. Madzia & M. Jung (Eds.), Pragmatism and embodied cognitive science: From bodily interaction to symbolic articulation (pp. 55–70). Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. J. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 7–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crippen, M. (2010). William James on belief: Turning Darwinism against empiricistic skepticism. Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society, 46, 477–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crippen, M. (2016). Dewey, enactivism and Greek thought. In R. Madzia & M. Jung (Eds.), Pragmatism and embodied cognitive science: From bodily interaction to symbolic Articulation (pp. 229–246). Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crippen, M. (2017). Embodied cognition and perception: Dewey, science and skepticism. Contemporary Pragmatism, 14, 121–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G.P. Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, S. (2006). Artistic expression and the hard case of pure music. In M. Kieran (Ed.), Contemporary debates in aesthetics and the philosophy of art (pp. 179–191). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1908/1931). The practical character of reality. In J. Dewey (Ed.), Philosophy and civilization (pp. 35–56). New York: Minton, Balch and Company.

  • Dewey, J. (1920). Reconstruction in philosophy. New York: Henry Holt and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1929). The quest for certainty. New York: Minton, Balch and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Minton, Balch and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York: Henry Holt and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. (1991). Being-in-the-world: A commentary on Heidegger’s Being and time, division I. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Du, S., Tao, Y., & Martinez, A. M. (2014). Compound facial expressions of emotion. PNAS, 111, E1454–E1462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dussutour, A., Latty, T., Beekman, M., & Simpson, S. J. (2010). Amoeboid organism solves complex nutritional challenges. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 460–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 19, pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (2003). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. (2010). Impulsive action, and motivation. Biological Psychology, 84, 570–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendron, M., Mesquita, B., & Barrett, L. (2013). Emotion perception: Putting the face in context. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 539–556). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1975). Pickford and the failure of experimental esthetics. Leonardo, 8, 319–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2001). Ecological psychology in context: James Gibson, Roger Barker, and the legacy of William James’ radical empiricism. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hufendiek, R. (2017). Affordances and the normativity of emotions. Synthese, 194, 4455–4476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1757). Four dissertations. London: A Millar, in the Stand.

  • Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jack, R. E., Sun, W., Delis, I., Garrod, O. G. B., & Schyns, P. G. (2016). Four not six: Revealing culturally common facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 708–730.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1879/1992). The sentiment of rationality. In G. E. Myers (Ed.), William James: Writings 18781899 (pp. 950–985). New York: Library of America.

  • Kaplan, S. (1987). Aesthetics, affect and cognition: Environmental preference from an evolutionary perspective. Environment and Behavior, 1, 4–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J. W. (2010). Doing things with music. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J., & Colombetti, G. (2018). Affective affordances and psychopathology. Discipline Filosofiche, 18, 221–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuleshov, L. (1929/1974). The art of the cinema. In R. Levaco (Ed. & Trans.), Kuleshov on film: Writings of Lev Kuleshov (pp. 147–158). Berkeley: University of California Press.

  • Mealey, L., & Theis, P. (1995). The relationship between mood and preferences among natural landscapes: An evolutionary perspective. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 247–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945/1962). Phenomenology of perception (Trans., C. Smith). London: Routledge.

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1947/1964). Film and the new psychology. In Sense and non-sense (Trans., H. Dreyfus & P. Dreyfus) (pp. 43–59). Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

  • Mobbs, D., Weiskopf, N., Lau, H. C., Featherstone, E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). The Kuleshov effect: The influence of contextual framing on emotional attributions. Scan, 1, 95–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, C. (2007). The musical representation: Meaning, ontology, and emotion. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, S., & Hensley, W. E. (1992). The Kuleshov effect: Recreating the classic experiment. Cinema Journal, 31, 59–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2004). Gut reactions: A perceptual theory of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D. R., & Linkenauger, S. A. (2013). Perception viewed as a phenotypic expression. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 171–197). London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pudovkin, V. (1926/1968). Film technique. In I. Montagu (Ed. & Trans.), Film technique and film acting (pp. 19–220). London: Vision Press.

  • Ratcliffe, M. (2002). Heidegger’s attunement and the neuropsychology of emotion. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1, 287–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe, M., & Broome, M. (forthcoming). Beyond ‘salience’ and ‘affordance’: Understanding anomalous experiences of significant possibilities. In S. Archer (Ed.), Salience: A philosophical inquiry. London: Routledge.

  • Reed, E. (1988). James J. Gibson and the psychology of perception. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, C. R., Latty, T., Dussutourc, A., & Beekmana, M. (2012). Slime mold uses an externalized spatial ‘memory’ to navigate in complex environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 17490–17494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riener, C. R., Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D., & Clore, G. L. (2011). An effect of mood on the perception of geographical slant. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 174–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. (2011). Expression theories. In T. Gracyk & A. Kania (Eds.), The Routledge companion to philosophy and Music (pp. 201–211). Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosar, W. H. (1994). Film music and Heinz Werner’s theory of physiognomic perception. Psychomusicology A Journal of Research in Music Cognition, 13, 154–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1993). Forced-choice response format in the study of facial expression. Motivation and Emotion, 17, 41–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (2006). Emotions are not modules. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 36, 53–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, H., Müllan, R. O., & Slaby, J. (2011). Emotions outside the box—The new phenomenology of feeling and corporeality. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10, 241–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Zadra, J., & Proffitt, D. R. (2010). Direct evidence for the economy of actions: Glucose and the perception of geographical slant. Perception, 39, 464–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slaby, J. (2014). Emotions and the extended mind. In M. Salmela & C. Scheeve (Eds.), Collective emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. L., Cottrell, G. W., Gosselin, F., & Schyns, P. G. (2005). Transmitting and decoding facial expressions. Psychological Science, 16, 184–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (2010). Forms of vitality: Exploring dynamic experience in psychology, the arts, psychotherapy, and development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. S., & McCarter, R. (1964). What and where are the primary effects: Some evidence for a theory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 18, 119–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivedi, S. (2011). Resemblance theories. In T. Gracyk & A. Kania (Eds.), The Routledge companion to philosophy and music (pp. 201–211). Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veltkamp, M., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2008). Perception in the service of goal pursuit: Motivation to attain goals enhances the perceived size of goal-instrumental objects. Social Cognition, 26, 720–736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallbott, H. (1988). In and out of context: Influences of facial expression and context on emotion attributions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 27(4), 357–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, S. (2010). Airport security: Intent to deceive. Nature, 465(7297), 412–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1927/1978). On physiognomic modes of perception and their experimental investigation. In S. S. Barten & M. B. Franklin (Eds.), Developmental processes: Heinz Werner’s selected writings (Vol. 1, pp. 149–152). New York: International Universities Press.

  • Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1938). General problems. In W. D. Ellis (Ed.), A source gestalt psychology (pp. 1–12). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zadra, J., Schnall, S., Weltman, A., & Proffitt, D. (2010). Direct physiological evidence for the economy of action: Bioenergetics and the perception of spatial layout. Journal of Vision, 10, 54.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Crippen.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Seeds of this article trace to enjoying movies with a friend during high school days, and then to more recent conference papers, improved by feedback. I also had help preparing video clips and with data collection. Accordingly, I have many acknowledgments: Sherifa Amin, Patrick Anderson, Evan Cameron, Matthew Dixon, Joerg Fingerhut, Michael Friendly, Senica Gonzalez, Alistair Isaac, Andrew Knoll, Lana Kühle, Hope Sample, Dave Ward and the two anonymous reviewers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Crippen, M. Aesthetics and action: situations, emotional perception and the Kuleshov effect. Synthese 198 (Suppl 9), 2345–2363 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02110-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02110-2

Keywords

Navigation