Skip to main content
Log in

Socio-economic status differences in mathematics accuracy, strategy use, and profiles in the early years of schooling

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Prior research has shown that students from low socio-economic status (SES) families are at greater risk for mathematics education and achievement, and these factors in turn, may impact their long-term well-being. This paper investigates differences in mathematics achievement by SES, using clinical interview (CI). Students in Kindergarten to Grade 3 were studied using cross-sectional and longitudinal data to examine differences in mathematics accuracy, strategy use, and profiles. Results indicate minor differences in overall accuracy and in overall profile scores. At the subtest level, differences were found for accuracy for all grade levels. There were significant differences in mathematics performance by SES. In particular, larger differences by SES were found in mathematics accuracy than in sensible strategy use. For profile scores, significant differences were found by SES using cross sectional data; moreover, students’ profile scores differed by subtests and sections of the CI. Longitudinal data analysis indicated significant changes in students’ profile scores; however, there were no differences in profile scores by SES over time. Curricular and assessment implications are discussed, with directions for future research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arias, C. A., Schorr, R. Y., & Warner, L. B. (2010). Using the clinical interview method to examine children’s mathematical thinking. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Denver, CO.

  • Caro, D. H. (2009). Socio-economic status and academic achievement tracjectories from childhood to adolescence. Canadian Journal of Education, 32(3), 558–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (2000). Analysis of clinical interviews: Foundations and model viability. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 547–589). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denton, K., & West, J. (2002). Children’s reading and mathematics achievement in kindergarten and first grade. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A. (2007). An interactional analysis of clinical interviewing. Cognition and Instruction, 25(4), 523–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., & Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428–1446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., & DeSoto, M. C. (2004). Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 121–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. P., & Pappas, S. (2004). SES, ethnic, and gender differences in young children’s informal addition and subtraction: a clinical interview investigation. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 171–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. P., Pappas, S., Lee, Y.-S., & Chiong, C. (2011). How did you get that answer? Computer assessments of young children’s mathematical minds in mCLASS math. In P. E. Noyce & D. T. Hickey (Eds.), New frontiers in formative assessment (pp. 49–67). Cambridge: Harvard Education Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. P., Pappas, S., & Seo, K.-H. (2001). Everyday mathematical knowledge: Asking young children what is developmentally appropriate. In S. L. Golbeck (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on early childhood education: Reframing dilemmas in research and practice (pp. 181–219). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. P., & Russell, R. L. (1981). Social class and racial influences on early mathematical thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46(Serial No. 193, No. 6).

  • Graham, T. A., Nash, C., & Paul, K. (1997). Young children’s exposure to mathematics: the child care context. Early Childhood Education Journal, 25(1), 31–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harwell, M., & LeBeau, B. (2010). Student eligibility for a free lunch as an SES measure in education research. Educational Researcher, 39(2), 120–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harwell, M. R., Maeda, Y., & Lee, K. (2004). Replicating and extending Whites (1982) Meta-analysis of the relationship between SES and student achievement. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.

  • Heckman, J. J. (2000). Policies to foster human capital. Research in Economics, 54(1), 3–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, M. (1986). Children and number: Difficulties in learning mathematics. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunting, R. P. (1997). Clinical interview methods in mathematics education research and practice. Journal of Mahtematical Behavior, 16(2), 145–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyson, M., & Woods, T. A. (2014). Practices, Knowledge and Beliefs about Professional Development. In H. P. Ginsburg, M. Hyson, & T. A. Woods (Eds.), Preparing Early Childhood Educators to Teach Math: Professional Development that Works (pp. 29–51). Baltimore: Brooks Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., Ramineni, C., & Watkins, M. W. (2010). Validating a number sense screening tool for use in Kindergarten and First Grade: prediction of mathematics proficiency in Third Grade. School Psychology Review, 39(2), 181–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N. C., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (1992). Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 28, 644–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N. C., Huttenlocher, L., & Levine, S. C. (1994). Assessing early arithmetic abilities: effects of verbal and nonverbal response types on the calculation performance of middle- and low-income children. Learning and Individual Differences, 6, 413–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Locuniak, M. N., & Ramineni, C. (2007). Predicting first grade math achievement from developmental number sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 22(1), 36–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Olah, L. N., & Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in Kindergarten: a longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Development, 77(1), 153–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2008). Development of number combination skill in the early school years: when do fingers help? Developmental science, 11(5), 662–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerkman, D. D., & Siegler, R. S. (1993). Individual differences and adaptive flexibility in lower-income children’s strategy choices. Learning and Individual Differences, 5(2), 113–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirk, G. E., Hunt, J. M., & Volkmar, F. (1975). Social class and preschool language skill: V. Cognitive and semantic mastery of number. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 93, 131–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, J. S., & Bisanz, J. (2000). Preschoolers doing arithmetic: the concepts are willing but the working memory is weak. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 105–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klibanoff, R. S., Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Hedges, L. V. (2006). Preschool children’s mathematical knowledge: the effect of teacher “math talk”. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 59–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(5), 178–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladson-Billings, G. (2006). From the achievement gap to the education debt: understanding achievement in U.S. Schools. Educational Researcher, 35(7), 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, J. (2002). Racial and ethnic achievement gap trends: reversing the progress toward equity? Educational Researcher, 31(1), 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, J.-S., & Bowen, N. K. (2006). Parent involvement, cultural capital, and the achievement gap among elementary school children. American Educational Research Journal, 43(2), 193–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Love, J. M., & Xue, Y. (2010). How early care and education programs 0-5 prepare children for Kindergarten: Is it enough? Paper presented at the Head Start’s 10th National Research Conference. DC: Washingon.

  • Lubienski, S. T., & Lubienski, C. (2006). School sector and academic achievement: a multilevel analysis of NAEP mathematics data. American Educational Research Journal, 43, 651–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maher, C. A., & Sigley, R. (2014). Task-based interviews in mathematics education. In S. Lerman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Mathematics Education (pp. 579–582). Netherlands: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pappas, S., Ginsburg, H. P., & Jiang, M. (2003). SES differences in young children’s metacognition in the context of mathematical problem solving. Cognitive Development, 18(3), 431–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B., Guberman, S. R., & Gearhart, M. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52(2, Serial No. 216).

  • Siegler, R. S. (1988). Individual differences in strategy choices: good students, not-so-good students, and perfectionists. Child Development, 59, 833–851.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: a meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P., & Klein, A. (1992). Economic and cultural influences on early mathematical development. In F. L. Parker, R. Robinson, S. Sombrano, C. Piotrowski, J. Hagen, S. Randolph, & A. Baker (Eds.), New directions in child and family research: Shaping Head Start in the 90s (pp. 440–443). New York: National Council of Jewish Women.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Young-Sun Lee.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 419 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lee, YS., Park, Y.S. & Ginsburg, H. Socio-economic status differences in mathematics accuracy, strategy use, and profiles in the early years of schooling. ZDM Mathematics Education 48, 1065–1078 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-016-0783-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-016-0783-y

Keywords

Navigation