Skip to main content
Log in

Does Context Matter? Effects of Robot Appearance and Reliability on Social Attention Differs Based on Lifelikeness of Gaze Task

  • Published:
International Journal of Social Robotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Social signals, such as changes in gaze direction, are essential cues to predict others’ mental states and behaviors (i.e., mentalizing). Studies show that humans can mentalize with nonhuman agents when they perceive a mind in them (i.e., mind perception). Robots that physically and/or behaviorally resemble humans likely trigger mind perception, which enhances the relevance of social cues and improves social-cognitive performance. The current experiments examine whether the effect of physical and behavioral influencers of mind perception on social-cognitive processing is modulated by the lifelikeness of a social interaction. Participants interacted with robots of varying degrees of physical (humanlike vs. robot-like) and behavioral (reliable vs. random) human-likeness while the lifelikeness of a social attention task was manipulated across five experiments. The first four experiments manipulated lifelikeness via the physical realism of the robot images (Study 1 and 2), the biological plausibility of the social signals (Study 3), and the plausibility of the social context (Study 4). They showed that humanlike behavior affected social attention whereas appearance affected mind perception ratings. However, when the lifelikeness of the interaction was increased by using videos of a human and a robot sending the social cues in a realistic environment (Study 5), social attention mechanisms were affected both by physical appearance and behavioral features, while mind perception ratings were mainly affected by physical appearance. This indicates that in order to understand the effect of physical and behavioral features on social cognition, paradigms should be used that adequately simulate the lifelikeness of social interactions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The p value has been adjusted using the Bonferroni procedure because two Levene’s tests—one for each Physical Humanness level—have been conducted.

  2. This manipulation was chosen based on previous research that has shown that people are highly sensitive in differentiating biological from non-biological motion [50, 51].

  3. The p-value has been adjusted using the Bonferroni procedure because two Levene’s tests—one for each Physical Humanness level—have been conducted.

  4. The p-values have been adjusted using the Bonferroni procedure because two Levene’s tests—one for each Physical Humanness level—have been conducted.

  5. Interestingly, the results of Experiment 4 show a descriptive difference such that gaze-cueing effects were overall larger increase. This is not surprising as previous studies have shown that including contextual information has a positive effect on gaze-cueing effects [17].

References

  1. Adolphs R (1999) Social cognition and the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci 3(12):469–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01399-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Emery NJ (2000) The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24(6):581–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn Sci 7(2):77–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Gray HM, Gray K, Wegner DM (2007) Dimensions of mind perception. Science 315(5812):619. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1134475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Waytz A, Gray K, Epley N, Wegner DM (2010) Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends Cogn Sci 14(8):383–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Wiese E, Metta G, Wykowska A (2017) Robots as intentional agents: using neuroscientific methods to make robots appear more social. Front Psychol 8:1663. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01663

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Friesen CK, Kingstone A (1998) The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by non-predictive gaze. Psychon Bull Rev 5(3):490–495. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208827

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Özdem C, Wiese E, Wykowska A, Müller H, Brass M, Van Overwalle F (2016) Believing androids—fMRI activation in the right temporo-parietal junction is modulated by ascribing intentions to non-human agents. Soc Neurosci 12(5):582–593. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1207702

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Teufel C, Fletcher PC, Davis G (2010) Seeing other minds: attributed mental states influence perception. Trends Cogn Sci 14(8):376–382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Wiese E, Wykowska A, Zwickel J, Müller HJ (2012) I see what you mean: how attentional selection is shaped by ascribing intentions to others. PLoS ONE 7(9):e45391. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045391

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Wiese E, Buzzell GA, Abubshait A, Beatty PJ (2018) Seeing minds in others: mind perception modulates low-level social-cognitive performance and relates to ventromedial prefrontal structures. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 18(5):837–856. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0608-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Wykowska A, Wiese E, Prosser A, Müller HJ (2014) Beliefs about the minds of others influence how we process sensory information. PLoS ONE 9(4):e94339. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Nummenmaa L, Calder AJ (2009) Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends Cogn Sci 13(3):135–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.12.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Pfeiffer UJ, Timmermans B, Bente G, Vogeley K, Schilbach L (2011) A non-verbal turing test: differentiating mind from machine in gaze-based social interaction. PLoS ONE 6(11):e27591. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027591

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Kompatsiari K, Ciardo F, Tikhanoff V, Metta G, Wykowska A (2018) On the role of eye contact in gaze cueing. Sci Rep 8(1):17842. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-36136-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Perez-Osorio J, Müller HJ, Wiese E, Wykowska A (2015) Gaze following is modulated by expectations regarding others’ action goals. PLoS ONE 10(11):e0143614. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143614

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Wiese E, Zwickel J, Müller HJ (2013) The importance of context information for the spatial specificity of gaze cueing. Atten Percept Psychophys 75(5):967–982. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0444-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Caruana N, de Lissa P, McArthur G (2017) Beliefs about human agency influence the neural processing of gaze during joint attention. Soc Neurosci 12(2):194–206. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1160953

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Deaner RO, Shepherd SV, Platt ML (2007) Familiarity accentuates gaze cuing in women but not men. Biol Lett 3(1):64–67. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2006.0564

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Admoni H, Bank C, Tan J, Toneva M, Scassellati B (2011) Robot gaze does not reflexively cue human attention. In: Proceedings of the 33rd annual conference of the cognitive science society, 1983–1988

  21. Admoni H, Scassellati B (2017) Social eye gaze in human-robot interaction: a review. J. Hum. Robot Interact. 6(1):25. https://doi.org/10.5898/JHRI.6.1.Admoni

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. DiSalvo CF, Gemperle F, Forlizzi J, Kiesler S (2002) All robots are not created equal: the design and perception of humanoid robot heads. In: Conference on designing interactive systems processes practices methods and techniques, pp 321–326. https://doi.org/10.1145/778712.778756

  23. Kiesler S, Powers A, Fussell SR, Torrey C (2008) Anthropomorphic interactions with a robot and robot–like agent. Soc Cogn 26(2):169–181. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.2.169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Looser CE, Wheatley T (2010) The tipping point of animacy: how, when, and where we perceive life in a face. Psychol Sci 21(12):1854–1862. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610388044

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Tung F (2011) Influence of gender and age on the attitudes of children towards humanoid robots. Hum Comput Interact IV. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-21619-0_76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Martini M, Buzzell G, Wiese E (2015) Agent appearance modulates mind attribution and social attention in human-robot interaction. Soc Robot 1:431–439. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-25554-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Wiese E, Weis PP (2020) It matters to me if you are human—examining categorical perception in human and nonhuman agents. Int J Hum Comput Stud 133:1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2019.08.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Wiese E, Wykowska A, Müller HJ (2014) What we observe is biased by what other people tell us: beliefs about the reliability of gaze behavior modulate attentional orienting to gaze cues. PLoS ONE 9(4):e94529. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Morewedge CK (2009) Negativity bias in attribution of external agency. J Exp Psychol Gen 138(4):535–545. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016796

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Morewedge CK, Preston J, Wegner DM (2007) Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. J Pers Soc Psychol 93(1):1–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Abell F, Happe F, Frith U (2000) Do triangles play tricks? attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn Dev 15(1):1–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00014-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Castelli F, Happé F, Frith U, Frith C (2013) Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. In: Social neuroscience: key readings, pp 155–170. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203496190

  33. Heider F, Simmel M (1944) An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am J Psychol 57(2):243–259. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Short E, Hart J, Vu M, Scassellati B (2010) No fair!! an interaction with a cheating robot. In 2010 5th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction (HRI), pp 219–226. https://doi.org/10.1109/HRI.2010.5453193

  35. Ciardo F, Beyer F, De Tommaso D, Wykowska A (2020) Attribution of intentional agency towards robots reduces one’s own sense of agency. Cognition 194:104109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Gray K, Knobe J, Sheskin M, Bloom P, Barrett LF (2011) More than a body: mind perception and the nature of objectification. J Pers Soc Psychol 101(6):1207–1220. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025883

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Abubshait A, Wiese E (2017) You look human, but act like a machine: agent appearance and behavior modulate different aspects of human–robot interaction. Front. Psychol. 8:1393. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01393

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Mandell AR, Smith MA, Martini MC, Shaw TH, Wiese E (2015) Does the presence of social agents improve cognitive performance on a vigilance task? Int. Conf. Soc. Robot. 1:421–430

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Admoni H, Dragan A, Srinivasa SS, Scassellati B (2014) Deliberate delays during robot-to-human handovers improve compliance with gaze communication. In: Proceedings of the 2014 ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction - HRI’14, pp 49–56. https://doi.org/10.1145/2559636.2559682

  40. Hungr CJ, Hunt AR (2012) Physical self-similarity enhances the gaze-cueing effect. Q J Exp Psychol 65(7):1250–1259. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.690769

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Waytz A, Heafner J, Epley N (2014) The mind in the machine: anthropomorphism increases trust in an autonomous vehicle. J Exp Soc Psychol 52:113–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.01.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Saygin AP, Chaminade T, Ishiguro H, Driver J, Frith C (2012) The thing that should not be: predictive coding and the uncanny valley in perceiving human and humanoid robot actions. SCAN 7:413–422. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr025

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. SR Research (2004) Experiment builder

  44. The Mathworks Inc (2015) Matlab

  45. Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10(4):433–436. https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Field AP (2005) Discovering statistics using SPSS: (and sex, drugs and rock’n’roll). SAGE, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  47. Feys J (2016) Nonparametric tests for the interaction in two-way factorial designs using R. R J 8(1):367. https://doi.org/10.32614/RJ-2016-027

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Ohman A (1988) Karolinska directed emotional faces. In: Psychology section. Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Hospital, S-171, 76

  49. Kätsyri J, Förger K, Mäkäräinen M, Takala T (2015) A review of empirical evidence on different uncanny valley hypotheses: support for perceptual mismatch as one road to the valley of eeriness. Front Psychol 6(MAR):1–16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Giese MA, Poggio T (2003) Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements. Nat Rev Neurosci 4(3):179–192. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1057

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Grossman ED, Blake R (2002) Brain areas active during visual perception of biological motion. Neuron 35:9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. SR Research (2010) Eyelink 1000

  53. Ristic J, Kingstone A (2005) Taking control of reflexive social attention. Cognition 94(3):B55–B65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.04.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Gobel MS, Tufft MRA, Richardson DC (2017) Social beliefs and visual attention: how the social relevance of a cue influences spatial orienting. Cogn Sci 42:161–185. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Kingstone A, Kachkovski G, Vasilyev D, Kuk M, Welsh TN (2019) Mental attribution is not sufficient or necessary to trigger attentional orienting to gaze. Cognition 189:35–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Capozzi F, Ristic J (2020) Attention and mentalizing? reframing a debate on social orienting of attention. Vis Cogn 28:97–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2020.1725206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Cole GG, Skarratt PA, Kuhn G (2016) Real person interaction in visual attention research. Eur Psychol 21(2):141–149. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000243

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Waytz A, Morewedge CK, Epley N, Monteleone G, Gao J-H, Cacioppo JT (2010) Making sense by making sentient: effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. J Pers Soc Psychol 99(3):410–435. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Schilbach L, Timmermans B, Reddy V, Costall A, Bente G, Schlicht T, Vogeley K (2013) Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav Brain Sci 36(4):393–414. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000660

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Redcay E, Kleiner M, Saxe R (2012) Look at this: the neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention. Front Hum Neurosci 6:169. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Lachat F, Conty L, Hugueville L, George N (2012) Gaze cueing effect in a face-to-face situation. J Nonverbal Behav 36(3):177–190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-012-0133-x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Caruana N, Brock J, Woolgar A (2015) A frontotemporoparietal network common to initiating and responding to joint attention bids. NeuroImage 108:34–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.041

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Schilbach L, Wilms M, Eickhoff SB, Romanzetti S, Tepest R, Bente G, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Vogeley K (2010) Minds made for sharing: initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. J Cogn Neurosci 22(12):2702–2715. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Thellman S, Silvervarg A, Gulz A, Ziemke T (2016) Physical vs. virtual agent embodiment and effects on social interaction. In: Traum D, Swartout W, Khooshabeh P, Kopp S, Scherer S, Leuski A (eds) Intelligent virtual agents, vol 10011. Springer, Berlin, pp 412–415. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47665-0_44

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  65. Mollahosseini A, Abdollahi H, Sweeny TD, Cole R, Mahoor MH (2018) Role of embodiment and presence in human perception of robots’ facial cues. Int J Hum Comput Stud 116:25–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2018.04.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Wainer J, Feil-Seifer DJ, Shell DA, Mataric MJ (2007) Embodiment and human–robot interaction: a task-based perspective. In: RO-MAN 2007—the 16th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication, pp 872–877. https://doi.org/10.1109/ROMAN.2007.4415207

  67. Lee KM, Jung Y, Kim J, Kim SR (2006) Are physically embodied social agents better than disembodied social agents?: the effects of physical embodiment, tactile interaction, and people’s loneliness in human-robot interaction. Int J Hum Comput Stud 64(10):962–973. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.05.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the hard-working research assistants that helped collect our sample.

Funding

This study was not funded by any grants.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AA and EW conceptualized the study. PW programmed the experiments. AA and PW collected and analyzed the data. AA, EW, and PW interpreted the results and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Abdulaziz Abubshait.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 47 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Abubshait, A., Weis, P.P. & Wiese, E. Does Context Matter? Effects of Robot Appearance and Reliability on Social Attention Differs Based on Lifelikeness of Gaze Task. Int J of Soc Robotics 13, 863–876 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-020-00675-4

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-020-00675-4

Keywords

Navigation