Skip to main content
Log in

Reversing Time and Size: Mutual Entailment of Nonarbitrary Temporal and Magnitude Relational Responding

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Responding to temporal relational statements that include the original events (e.g., A. .. B) in a reversed order (e.g., “B after A”) is less accurate and more time-consuming than responding to such statements when they retain the original order of presentation (e.g., “A before B”). The current study assessed whether this effect was limited to temporal relational responding by estimating the effect of reversal on magnitude statements (e.g., “B bigger than A”) as well as temporal statements. Participants (N = 40) completed temporal and magnitude relational judgement tasks in blocks consisting of a training phase and a testing phase. The order of relational tasks was counterbalanced across participants; participants learned the second type of relational task faster than the first. During testing, reversal of the order of stimuli in both temporal and magnitude relations reduced accuracy and increased response latencies suggesting that the reversal effect was not limited to temporal relations. The findings support the position that a general relational effect, such as mutual entailment, may underlie the increased difficulty of reversed temporal relational statements.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agresti, A. (2013). Categorical data analysis (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Regan, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Commins, S., Walsh, D., Stewart, I., ... Dymond, S. (2005). Relating derived relations as a model of analogical reasoning: Reaction times and event-related potentials. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 84, 435–451. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2005.79-04.

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training intervention to raise intelligence quotients: A pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61(2), 173–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. (1971). On the acquisition of the meaning of before and after. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 266–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11(1), 51–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1994). A transfer of self-discrimination response functions through equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 251–267. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaeb.1994.62-251.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1995). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the derived stimulus relations of sameness, more than, and less than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 163–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1996). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness and opposition. The Psychological Record, 46(2), 271–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2013). Advances in relational frame theory: Research and application. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrell, F. E. (2017). Hmisc: Harrell miscellaneous. (R package version 4.0-3). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmisc

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, J. M., O’ Hora, D. P., Leslie, J. C., & Smyth, S. (2012). Sequential responding in accordance with temporal relational cues: A comparison of “before” and “after”. The Psychological Record, 62(3), 463–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, J. M., Smyth, S., O’ Hora, D. P., & Leslie, J. C. (2014). The effect of before and after instructions on the speed of sequential responding. The Psychological Record, 64(2), 311–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lidji, P., Palmer, C., Peretz, I., & Morningstar, M. (2011). Listeners feel the beat: Entrainment to English and French speech rhythms. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1035–1041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipkins, R., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Longitudinal study of the development of derived relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 201–239. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1993.1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., DeGutis, J., & Chenevert, T. L. (2000). Order information in working memory: fMRI evidence for parietal and prefrontal mechanisms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 130–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGreal, C., Hyland, J., O’Hora, D., & Hogan, M. (2016). Mutual entailment of temporal relations in younger and older adults: Reversing order judgments. The Psychological Record, 66(3), 419–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munnelly, A., Dymond, S., & Hinton, E. C. (2010). Relational reasoning with derived comparative relations: A novel model of transitive inference. Behavioral Processes, 85, 8–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.05.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. (2004). Derived relational networks and control by novel instructions: A possible model of generative verbal responding. The Psychological Record, 54(3), 437–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hora, D., & Maglieri, K. A. (2006). Goal statements and goal-directed behavior: A relational frame account of goal setting in organizations. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 26(1), 131–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Hora, D., Peláez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & Chaudhary, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58(4), 569–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Psychology Software Tools. (2004). E-Prime (vers. 2) [computer software]. Sharpsburg: PA: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/

  • Reilly, T., Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2005). The effect of training structure on the latency of responses to a five-term linear chain. The Psychological Record, 55(2), 233–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz, F. J., & Luciano, C. (2011). Cross-domain analogies as relating derived relations among two separate relational networks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 369–385. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaeb.2011.95-369.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, D., & Hayes, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56(3), 519–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2004). A functional-analytic model of analogy using the relational evaluation procedure. The Psychological Record, 54(4), 531–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). MASS: Modern applied statistics with S (R package 4). New York, NY: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.).: UK manual. London, UK: Psychological Corporation.

  • Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York, NY: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Hyland.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brassil, N., Hyland, J., O’Hora, D. et al. Reversing Time and Size: Mutual Entailment of Nonarbitrary Temporal and Magnitude Relational Responding. Psychol Rec 69, 95–105 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0323-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0323-y

Keywords

Navigation