Skip to main content
Log in

A Cognitive Model of Selective Processing in Anxiety

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Anxiety states are associated with increasedattention to threat and a greater likelihood of reachinga pessimistic interpretation of ambiguous events.Existing models of this selective processing possess features that are difficult to reconcile withcurrent experimental findings. In this paper we build onthese earlier ideas to develop a new model,incorporating adaptations that allow it to accountbetter for the accumulating data. Essential featuresare that attributes or meanings of stimuli are processedin parallel and compete for attentional resources. Inputfrom a threat evaluation system (TES) strengthens activation of threat-related attributes, to anextent influenced by anxiety level. Such activation canbe countered, within limits, by voluntary task-relatedeffort, and the balance between these opposing influences determines the extent of anyattentional or interpretative bias seen. Such a model isplausible from an evolutionary perspective and isconsistent with neurological evidence concerning theacquisition and extinction of aversiveconditioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Amir, N., McNally, F. J., Riemann, B. C., Burns, J., Loenz, M., & Mullen, J. T. (1996). Suppression of the emotional Stroop effect by increased anxiety in patients with social phobia. Behavior Research and Therapy, 34, 945–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton, M. E. (1994). Context, ambiguity, and classical conditioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 49–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., & Mathews, A. (1983). Negative self-schemata in clinical depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22,173–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. (1988). Anxiety and attentional bias: State and trait. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 165–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryne, A., & Eysenck, M. W. (1995). Trait anxiety, anxious mood, and threat detection. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 549–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., & McGaugh, J. L. (1994). b-Adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature, 371,702–704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M. G., & Castillo, M. D. (1997). Mood congruent bias in interpretation of ambiguity: Strategic processes and temporary activation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 50A, 163–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M. G., Eysenck, M. W., & Estevez, A. (1994). Ego-threat interpretative bias in test anxiety: On-line inferences. Cognition and Emotion, 8,127–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T. H., & Dagenbach, D. (1990). Semantic priming and repetition from masked words: Evidence for a center-surround attentional mechanism in perceptual recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16,341–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McCleland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 99,45–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corr, P. J., Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1997). Personality, punishment and procedural learning: A test of J. A. Gray's anxiety theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,337–344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and attention: Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 66,1128–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Mogg, K., May, J., Richards, A., & Mathews, A. (1991). Bias in interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100,144–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1996). Selective processing of threatening words in anxiety: The role of awareness. Cognition and Emotion, 10,449–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fulcher, E. (1995). The construction of evaluative maps: Affective computations in the amygdala. In B. Kappen & S. Gielen (Eds.), Neural Networks: Artificial Intelligence and Industrial Applications(pp. 95–99). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. E. (1990). Investigating differences in comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 430–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1982). The Neuropsychology of anxiety. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress.Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Klinger, M. R., & Liu, T. J. (1989). Unconscious processing of dichotically masked words. Memory and Cognition, 17, 35–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Klinger, M. R., & Schuh, E. S. (1995). Activation by marginally perceptible (“subliminal”) stimuli: Dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 22–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (1990). Vivid memories of emotional events: The accuracy of remembered minutiae. Memory and Cognition, 18, 496–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Interpretive inference s when reading about emotional events. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35, 1123–1132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T.H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention memory and language(pp. 53–112). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W. J., & Nadel, L. (1985). Stress-induced recovery of fears and phobias. Psychological Review, 92, 512–531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1995). International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Gainesville: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Cohen, I. L. (1993). Anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguity: A text comprehension study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102,238–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macleod, C., and Hagan, R. (1992). Individual difference s in the selective processing of threatening information, and emotional responses to a stressful life event. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30,151–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 38,659–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1991). Biased cognitive operations in anxiety: Accessibility of information or assignment of processing priorities. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29,599–610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95,15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & McLaughlin, K. (1995). Implicit and explicit memory bias in anxiety: A conceptual replication. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 1–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Rutherford, E. M. (1992). Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: Mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus materials. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30,479–491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15,238–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 455–468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., & Fulcher, E. (1997). Cognitive biases in anxiety and attention to threat. Trends in Cognitive Science, 1, 340–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45,25–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., May, J., Mogg, K., & Eysenck, M. (1990). Attentional bias in anxiety: Selective search or defective filtering? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 166–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & Milroy, R. (1994). Processing of emotional meaning in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 535–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Mogg, K., Kentish, J., & Eysenck, M. (1995). Effect of psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Research and Therapy, 33,293–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Mogg K., May J., & Eysenck, M. (1989). Implicit and explicit memory bias in anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 236–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Richards, A., & Eysenck, M. (1989). Interpretation of homophones related to threat in anxiety states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 31–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & Sebastian S. (1993). Suppression of emotional Stroop effects by fear-arousal. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 517–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., & Harley, T. A. (1996). Connectionist models of emotional distress and attentional bias. Cognition and Emotion, 10,561–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattia, J. I., Heimber, R. G., & Hope, D. A. (1993). The revised Stroop colour-naming task in social phobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31,305–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36,809–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Millar, N., & White, J. (1995). A follow-up study of cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33,927–935.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Mathews, A., & Eysenck, M. (1992). Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 149–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Mathews, A., Eysenck, M., & May, J. (1991). Biased cognitive operations in anxiety: Artefact, processing priorities or attentional search? Behavior Research and Therapy, 29, 459–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M. (1990). Implicit perception of affective information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 505–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nugent, K., & Mineka, S. (1994). The effect of high and low trait anxiety on implicit and explicit memory tasks. Cognition and Emotion, 8,147–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions(pp. 511–536). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,231–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1998). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: Expectancies for aversive outcomes following non-recognized fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127,69–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Power, M., & Dalgleish, T. (1997). Cognition and emotion: From order to disorder.Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1990). Fear and courage. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S., & Lopatka, C. (1986). Do fears summate? III. Behavior Research & Therapy, 24, 653–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, A., & French, C. C. (1992). An anxiety-related bias in semantic activation when processing threat/neutral homographs. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 45,503–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., Marsh, R. L., & Brody, N. (1984). Critical importance of exposure duration for affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10,465–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, G. B., & Kang, H. (1994). Inhibitory processes in the recognition of homograph meanings. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention memory and language(pp. 359–382). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103,193–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toth, J. P., & Reingold, E. M. (1995). Beyond perception: Conceptual contributions to unconscious influences of memory. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Implicit cognition(pp. 41–84). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (1993). The perception of features and objects In A. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness, and control(pp. 5–35). Oxford, U.K.: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Attentions and emotion: A clinical perspective.Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders.Chichester, U.K.: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders.(2nd ed.) Chichester, U.K.: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. ( 1996 ). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120,3–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., & Schwarz, N. (1997). Subliminal affective priming resists attributional interventions. Cognition and Emotion, 11,433–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yiend, J., Mathews, A., Bradley, B., & Mogg, K. (1998). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Zinbarg, R., & Revelle, W. (1989). Personality and conditioning: A test of four models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 301–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B. A Cognitive Model of Selective Processing in Anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research 22, 539–560 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018738019346

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018738019346

Navigation