Skip to main content

Creative Cognition in Gifted Youth

  • Chapter
International Handbook on Giftedness

Abstract

We focus on the cognitive processes that gifted adolescents bring to bear on creative tasks, particularly open-ended tasks that involve generating novel, candidate ideas and developing those ideas into creative products. We use the Geneplore model of creative cognition as an orienting framework and focus on processes of divergent production, problem finding and construction, and retrieval of conceptual stored knowledge at different levels of abstraction. We consider the extent to which gifted individuals employ those processes differently than individuals who have not been so identified. However, we also incorporate findings regarding motivation, the proclivity toward innovative action, implicit theories, and creative self-concept because, although a complete understanding of creative cognition in the gifted requires attention to cognitive processes, it also requires a consideration of factors outside the purely cognitive realm.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 669.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 849.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 849.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ablard, K. E., & Mills, J. A. (1996). Implicit theories of intelligence and self-perceptions of academically talented adolescents and children. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, 137–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, R. S., & Runco, M. A. (1989). Independence and the creative potential of gifted and exceptionally gifted boys. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18, 221–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. A. (1986). Protocol analysis: A methodology for exploring the information processing of gifted students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 30, 28–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barden, R. C., Zelko, F. A., Duncan, S. W., & Masters, J. C. (1980). Children’s consensual knowledge about the experiential determinants of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 968–976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, F., & Harrington, D. M. (1981). Creativity, intelligence, and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 439–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basadur, M. (1994). Managing the creative process in organizations. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 237–268). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basadur, M. (1997). Organizational development interventions for enhancing creativity in the workplace. Journal of Creative Behavior, 31, 59–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bempechat, J., London, P., & Dweck, C. S. (1991). Children’s conceptions of ability in major domains: An interview and experimental study. Child Study Journal, 21, 11–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. T. (1989). Creativity: What are we to measure? In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caropreso, E., & White, C. (1994). Analogical reasoning and giftedness: A comparison between identified gifted and nonidentified children. Journal of Educational Research, 87(5), 271–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chand, I., & Runco, M. A. (1992). Problem finding skills as components of the creative process. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 155–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Czikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture, and person: A systems view of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikzentmihalyi, M., & Getzels, J. W. (1971). Discovery-oriented behavior and the originality of creative products: A study with artists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 47–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldhusen, J. F., Treffinger, D. J., Von Mendfrans, A. P., & Ferris, D. R. (1971). The relationship between academic grades and divergent thinking scores derived from four different methods of testing. The Journal of Experimental Education, 40, 35–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallucci, NT, Middleton, G, & Kline, A (1999). The independence of creative potential and behavior disorders in gifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 43, 194–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. W., & Smilansky, J. (1983). Individual differences in pupil perceptions of school problems. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53, 307–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groboroz, M., & Necka, E. (2003). Creativity and cognitive control: Explorations of generation and evaluation skills. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 183–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, D. M. (1975). Effects of explicit instructions to “be creative” on the psychological meaning of divergent thinking test scores. Journal of Personality, 43, 434–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. A. (1988). The conditions of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hocevar, D. (1979). The development of the Creative Behavior Inventory. Paper presented at the Rocky Mountain Psychological Association. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 170 350)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, S. M. (1994). Scientific problem finding in gifted fifth-grade students. Roeper Review, 16(3), 156–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, S. M., & Feldhusen, J. F. (1990). The scientific hypothesis formulation of gifted ninth-grade students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 838–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, S. M., & Feldhusen, J. F. (1994). Scientific problem solving and problem finding: A theoretical model. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 201–219). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kershner, J. R., & Ledger, G. (1985). Effects of sex, intelligence and style of thinking on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1033–1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knight, B. A. (1995). The influence of locus of control on gifted and talented students. Gifted Education International, 11, 31–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kulikowich, J., & Alexander, P. (1990). The effects of gender, ability, and grade on analogy performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 15(4), 364–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., & Lopez, D. F. (1997). Regularities in the development of children’s causality beliefs about school performance across six sociocultural contexts. Developmental Psychology, 33, 165–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D., & Sternberg, R. (1986). Analogical reasoning with novel concepts: Differential attention of intellectually gifted and nongifted children to relevant and irrelevant novel stimuli. Cognitive Development, 1(1), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mönks, F. M., & Mason, E. J. (1993). Developmental theories and giftedness. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Mönks, et al. (Eds.) International handbook of research and development of giftedness and talent. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mooney, R. L. (1963). A conceptual model for integrating four approaches to the identification of creative talent. In C. W. Taylor, & F. Barron (Eds.), Scientific creativity: It’s recognition and development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mumford, M. D., Baughman, W. A., Costanza, D. P., Uhlman, C. Z., & Connelly, M. S. (1993). Developing creative capabilities: Implications of cognitive processing models. Roeper Review, 16, 16–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Uhlman, C. E., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic model s of creative thought. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 91–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mumford, M. D., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Redmond, M. R. (1994). Problem construction and cognition: Applying problem representations in ill-defined problems. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 3–39). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murrone, J., & Gynther, M. D. (1989). Implicit theories or halo effect? Conceptions about children’s intelligence. Psychological Reports, 65, 1187–1193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murrone, J. & Gynther, M. D. (1991). Teacher’s implicit ‘theories’ of children’s intelligence. Psychological Reports, 69, 1195–1201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renzulli, J. (1986). The three-ring conception of giftedness: A developmental model for creative productivity. In R. J. Sternberg and J. A. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappa, 42, 305-310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. (1984). Teachers’ judgments of creativity and social validation of divergent thinking tests. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59(3), 711–717.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. (1985). Reliability and convergent validity of ideational flexibility as a function of academic achievement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61(3), 1075–1081.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. (1986a). Divergent thinking and creative performance in gifted and nongifted children. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 46(2), 375–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (1986b). Maximal performance on divergent thinking tests by gifted, talented and nongifted children. Psychology in the Schools, 23, 308–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (1987). The generality of creative performance in gifted and nongifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 31, 121–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (1990). Implicit theories and ideational creativity. In M. A. Runco, & R. S. Albert (Eds.), Theories of creativity. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (1993). Divergent thinking, creativity, and giftedness. Gifted Child Quarterly, 37, 16–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M., & Albert, R. (1986). Exceptional giftedness in early adolescence and intrafamilial divergent thinking. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 15(4), 335–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A., & Bahleda, M. D. (1986). Implicit theories of artistic, scientific and everyday creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 20, 93–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A., & Chand, I. (1994). Conclusions concerning problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 217–290). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A., & Chand, I. (1995). Cognition and creativity. Educational Psychology Review, 7, 243–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A., McCarthy, K. A., & Svenson, E. (1993). Judgments of the creativity of artwork from students and professional artists. The Journal of Psychology, 128, 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M., & Nemiro, J. (1994). Problem finding, creativity, and giftedness. Roeper Review, 16(4), 235–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. N., Ward, T. B., & Dodds, R. A. (2000). Beliefs, activities, and attitudes related to creativity in gifted adolescents. Paper presented at the 108th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association; Washington D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders Wickes, K. N. (2004). Developing creative competencies in adolescence: Comparing implicit theories and creative abilities in the gifted and non-gifted adolescent. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section B, 64, 4090.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders Wickes, K. N. & Ward, T. B. (2006). Measuring gifted adolescents’ implicit theories of creativity. Roeper Review, 28, 131–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Stevens, T. P. M., & Carr, M. (1997). Metacognitive knowledge of gifted children and nonidentified children in early elementary school. Gifted Child Quarterly, 41, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shavinina, L. V., & Kholodnaja, M. A. (1996). The cognitive experience as a psychological basis of intellectual giftedness. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 20, 3–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Song, K., & Porath, M. (2005). Common and domain-specific cognitive characteristics of gifted students: an integrated model of human abilities. High Ability Studies, 16, 229–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 607–627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1988). A three-facet model of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 125–147). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B. E., Ketron, J. L., & Bernstein, M. (1981). People’s conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 37–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1993). Creative giftedness: A multivariate investment approach. Gifted Child Quarterly, 37, 7–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concepts of creativity: Prospect and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, C. W. (1988). Various approaches to and definitions of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.). The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tidwell, R. (1980). Gifted students’ self-images as a function of identification procedure, race, and sex. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 5, 57–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1974). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treffinger, D. J., Isaksen, S. G., & Dorval, K. B. (1994). Creative problem solving: An overview. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 223–236). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Garderen, D., & Montague, M. (2003). Visual-spatial representation, mathematical problem solving, and students of varying abilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 18(4), 246–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children: A study of the creativity-intelligence distinction. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wakefield, J. F. (1985). Towards creativity: Problem finding in a divergent-thinking exercise. Child Study Journal, 15, 265–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T. B. (1994). Structured imagination: The role of conceptual structure in exemplar generation. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T. B. (1995). What’s old about new ideas? In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Finke, R. A. (1999a). Creative cognition. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T. B., Saunders, K. N., & Dodds, R. A. (1999b). Creative cognition in gifted adolescents, Roeper Review, 21, 260–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T. B., Patterson, M. J., Sifonis, C. M., Dodds, R. A., & Saunders, K. N. (2002). The role of graded category structure in imaginative thought. Memory & Cognition, 30, 199–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T. B., Patterson, M. J. & Sifonis, C. (2004). The role of specificity and abstraction in creative idea generation. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.) Handbook of child psychology, (5th ed. Vol. 2). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winner, E. (2000). Giftedness: Current theory and research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 153–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas B. Ward .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Saunders Wickes, K.N., Ward, T.B. (2009). Creative Cognition in Gifted Youth. In: Shavinina, L.V. (eds) International Handbook on Giftedness. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6162-2_17

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics