Skip to main content

Flow and Its Affective, Cognitive, and Performance-Related Consequences

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Advances in Flow Research

Abstract

This chapter focuses on the specific autotelic quality and the affective, cognitive, and performance-related consequences of the flow experience. Research findings documenting a positive relationship between skills–demands compatibility (the central precondition of flow experiences) and components of an autotelic experience (intrinsic motivation, enjoyment, and involvement) are discussed. Besides, possible consequences of flow experiences are addressed. A review of currently available findings indicates that flow may foster positive affect and even lead to enhanced performance. Unfortunately, the findings, which are mostly correlational in nature, do not provide conclusive evidence regarding the consequences related to flow experiences—reflecting the fact that the empirical analysis of flow experiences is quite complex. Important intricacies of flow research and theorizing and their implications are discussed—specifically, the lack of methods to test for causal effects of flow experiences and the tendency to equate flow experience with skills–demands compatibility.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Csikszentmihalyi and others usually refer to “challenges” instead of “demands.” Also, in most flow studies, “challenges” are measured. However, the use of the term “challenge” in the conceptualization and measurement of skills–demands compatibility is problematic because from the viewpoint of motivational psychology, challenges occur as a consequence of compatibility between demands of the situation and the individual’s skills (cf. Rheinberg 2008). Furthermore, it should be noted that the term “challenge” typically implies the notion that an individual perceives the relevant situation as stimulating, whereas the term “demands” does not include an implication regarding the individual’s experiential state. Accordingly, we argue that to put challenges in relation to skills (a common praxis in flow research) is conceptually not particularly meaningful. Therefore, we prefer the term “demands” and will use it in the course of the chapter.

  2. 2.

    However, as persistence in a task is often related to ego-involvement rather than previous enjoyment (e.g., Ryan et al. 1991), it is usually used in combination with self-report measures and should not be understood as a replacement of them.

  3. 3.

    Inconsistencies in ESM studies could also be attributed to the measurement of “challenges” instead of “demands.” This especially holds for the cross-cultural study by Moneta (2004 b) as the term “challenge” may be interpreted differently in different cultures.

References

  • Abuhamdeh, S., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation orientations in the competitive context: An examination of person-situation interactions. Journal of Personality, 77, 1615–1635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bakker, A. B. (2008). The work-related flow inventory: Construction and initial validation of the WOLF. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72, 400–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V., Rabe, C., & Wölk, M. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: Does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 665–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., van Baaren, R. B., & Bargh, J. A. (2006). Linking automatic evaluation to mood and information processing style: Consequences for experienced affect, impression formation, and stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 135, 70–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., Wigand, R. T., & Nilan, M. (2000). Exploring web users optimal flow experiences. Information Technology and People, 13, 263–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, S. G., & Haworth, J. T. (1994). “Flow” experience in the daily lives of sixth-form college students. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 511–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety: Experiencing flow in work and play. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1982). Toward a psychology of optimal experience. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 13–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow. The psychology of engagement with everyday life. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy? American Psychologist, 54, 821–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the Experience Sampling Method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 529–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1993). The measurement of flow in everyday life: Toward a theory of emergent motivation. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1992: Developmental perspectives on motivation, current theory and research in motivation, 1992 (Vol. 40, pp. 57–97). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented teenagers: A longitudinal study of their development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Schiefele, U. (1994). Interest and the quality of experience in classrooms. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 10, 251–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1972). Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic reinforcement, and inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 113–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B., & Leone, D. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 39–80). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985a). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behaviour. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985b). The general causality scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delle Fave, A., & Bassi, M. (2000). The quality of experience in adolescents’ daily life: Developmental perspectives. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 126, 347–367.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Demerouti, E. (2006). Job characteristics, flow and performance: The moderating role of conscientiousness. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 266–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberger, R., Jones, J. R., Stinglhamber, F., Shanock, L., & Randall, A. T. (2005). Flow experiences at work: For high need achievers alone? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 755–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eklund, R. C. (1996). Preparing to compete: A season-long investigation with collegiate wrestlers. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 111–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engeser, S., & Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, moderators of challenge-skill-balance and performance. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 158–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engeser, S., Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Bischoff, J. (2005). Motivation, Flow-Erleben und Lernleistung in universitären Lernsettings [Motivation, flow experience and performance in learning settings at university]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 19, 159–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, B. S., & Avsec, A. (2007). The experience of flow and subjective well-being of music students. Horizons of Psychology, 17, 5–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fullagar, C. J., & Mills, M. (2008). Motivation and flow: Toward an understanding of the dynamics of the relation in architecture students. The Journal of Psychology, 142, 533–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, D. L., Katz, L. B., & Leirer, V. O. (1980). Cognitive representation of personality impressions: Organizational processes in first impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1050–1063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haworth, J. T., & Hill, S. (1992). Work, leisure and psychological well-being in a sample of young adults. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 2, 147–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1989). Motivation und Handeln [Motivation and action]. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hektner, J. M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). A longitudinal exploration of flow an intrinsic motivation in adolescents. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American education research association. New York: Alfred Sloan Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heo, J., Lee, Y., Pedersen, P. M., & McCormick, B. P. (2010). Flow experience in the daily lives of older adults: An analysis of the interaction between flow, individual differences, serious leisure, location, and social context. Canadian Journal on Aging, 29, 411–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huntsinger, J. R., Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and global-local focus: Priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing. Emotion, 10, 722–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, S. A., Kimiecik, J. C., Ford, S. K., & Marsh, H. W. (1998). Psychological correlates of flow in sport. Journal of Sport & Experience Psychology, 20, 358–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, S. A., & Roberts, G. C. (1992). Positive performance states of athletes: Toward a conceptual understanding of peak performance. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 156–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, S. A., Thomas, P. R., Marsh, H. W., & Smethurst, C. J. (2001). Relationship between flow, self-concept, psychological skills, and performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 129–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2008). Flow and regulatory compatibility: An experimental approach to the flow model of intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 196–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., Bless, H., & Blomann, F. (2011a). Flow and the tunnel vision: Cognitive consequences of a skills-demands-compatibility. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., Bless, H., Blomann, F., & Kleinbohl, D. (2011b). Physiological aspects of flow experiences: Skills-demand-compatibility effects on heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 849–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., & Blomann, F. (2008). Locus of control and the flow experience: An experimental analysis. European Journal of Personality, 22, 589–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., & Landhäußer, A. (2012). Im Flow sein: Experimentelle Analysen des Zustands optimaler Beanspruchung [Experiencing flow: Experimental analyses of the experiential state resulting from optimal task demands]. Psychologische Rundschau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., Landhäußer, A., & Fischer, J. (2011c). Effects of a skills-demands-compatibility: Flow experiences and physiological reactions in an aversive activity. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., Ringelhan, S., & Blomann, F. (2011d). Does skills-demands-compatibility result in intrinsic motivation? Experimental test of a basic notion proposed in the theory of flow-experiences. Manuscript under review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. Handbook of Social Psychology, 1, 233–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (1999). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Interesse. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45, 387–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landhäußer, A., Keller, J., & Frommherz, A. (2011). The paradox of work reconsidered: A systematic test based on the Day Reconstruction Method. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E. (2005). The relationship of motivation and flow experience to academic procrastination in university students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 5–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, R., Byrne, C., & Carlton, L. (2006). Creativity and flow in musical composition: An empirical investigation. Psychology of Music, 34, 292–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massimini, F., & Carli, M. (1988). The systematic assessment of flow in daily experience. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 266–287). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Massimini, F., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Delle Fave, A. (1988). Flow and biocultural evolution. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 60–81). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McInman, A. D., & Grove, J. R. (1991). Peak moments in sport: A literature review. Quest, 43, 333–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moller, A. C., Meier, B. P., & Wall, R. D. (2010). Developing an experimental induction of flow: Effortless action in the lab. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 191–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G. B. (2004a). The flow experience across cultures. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 115–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G. B. (2004b). The flow model of state intrinsic motivation in Chinese: Cultural and personal moderators. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2, 181–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of perceived challenges and skills on the quality of subjective experience. Journal of Personality, 64, 275–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Models of concentration in natural environments: A comparative approach based on streams of experiential data. Social Behavior and Personality, 27, 603–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, Y. (1988). Optimal experience and the uses of talent. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 319–326). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Flow theory and research. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 195–206). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partington, S., Partington, E., & Olivier, S. (2009). The dark side of flow: A qualitative study of dependence in big wave surfing. The Sport Psychologist, 23, 170–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological Assessment, 5, 164–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Percival, G., Crous, F., & Schepers, J. M. (2003). Cognitive potencial and job complexity as predictors of flow. Journal of Industrial Psychology, 29, 60–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, R. W. (2005). High performance experience in the design of national security. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50, 610–641.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (2008). Intrinsic motivation and flow-experience. In H. Heckhausen & J. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 323–348). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., Manig, Y., Kliegl, R., Engeser, S., & Vollmeyer, R. (2007). Flow bei der Arbeit, doch Glück in der Freizeit. Zielausrichtung, Flow und Glücksgefühle [Flow during work but happiness during leisure time: Goals, flow-experience, and happiness]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 51, 105–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). Interest and self-regulation: The relation between havingto and wanting to. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 343–374). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schallberger, U., & Pfister, R. (2001). Flow-Erleben in Arbeit und Freizeit: Eine Untersuchung zum ‘Paradox der Arbeit’ mit der Experience Sampling Method (ESM) [Flow experiences in work and leisure: An experience sampling study about the paradox of work]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 45, 176–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., & Roussakis, E. (2006a). Die Bedingungen des Flow-Erlebens in einer experimentellen Spielsituation. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 214, 207–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., & Roussakis, E. (2006b). Die Bedingungen des Flow-Erlebens in einer experimentellen Spielsituation [Experimental conditions for experiencing flow in computer games]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie mit Zeitschrift für angewandte Psychologie und Sprache & Kognition, 214, 207–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J. (2007). Arousal of flow experience in a learning setting and its effects on exam performance and affect. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 21, 217–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, J., & Brunner, S. (2009). The rewarding effect of flow experience on performance in a marathon race. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 10, 168–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). The independent effects of goal contents and motives on well-being: It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475–486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Schneider, B., & Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engagement in high school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory. School Psychology Quarterly, 18, 158–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shin, D. C., & Johnson, D. M. (1978). Avowed happiness as an overall assessment of the quality of life. Social Indicators Research, 5(475), 492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, G. L., Kimiecik, J. C., Daniels, J., & Jackson, S. A. (1995). Psychological antecedents of flow in recreational sport. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 125–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoll, O., & Lau, A. (2005). Flow-Erleben beim Marathonlauf – Zusammenhänge mit Anforderungspassung und Leistung. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 12, 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A. J. (1988). Self-esteem and optimal experience. In M. Csikszentmihalyi (Ed.), Flow: The psychology of optimal experience (pp. 327–341). New York: Harper and Row.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Dynamic psychology. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne Landhäußer .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Landhäußer, A., Keller, J. (2012). Flow and Its Affective, Cognitive, and Performance-Related Consequences. In: Engeser, S. (eds) Advances in Flow Research. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics