Skip to main content

Measures of Expressive Language

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Assessing Literacy in Deaf Individuals

Abstract

This chapter presents the data related to expressive language functioning for the VL2 Psychometric Toolkit Project. The data were generated using two measures of phonemic fluency, F–A–S and the sign-based analogue 5–1–U developed for this study, and two semantic category fluency tasks, animal and food naming. The latter tasks were administered following both of the phonemic fluency tasks to investigate the impacts of linguistic priming. The sample was comprised of deaf college students evaluated as part of the VL2 Psychometric Toolkit. In addition to providing descriptive statistics, correlations with a brief measure of nonverbal intelligence and measures of executive, visuospatial, memory, and other aspects of linguistic functioning and academic achievement administered concurrently as part of the VL2 Toolkit are presented. The relationships observed are discussed in the context of both research in the general population and, when available, previous research with deaf individuals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Anderson, P. (2008). Towards a developmental model of executive function. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, & P. J. Anderson (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 3–21). New York: Taylor and Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod, B. N., & Henry, R. R. (1992). Age-related performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting, Similarities, and Controlled Oral Word Association Tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 16–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barry, D., Bates, M. E., & Labouvie, E. (2008). FAS and CFL forms of verbal fluency differ in difficulty: A meta-analytic study. Applied Neuropsychology, 15(2), 97–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Battison, R. (1978). Lexical borrowing in American Sign Language. Silver Spring, MD: Linstok Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtoldt, H. P., Benton, A. L., & Fogel, M. L. (1962). An application of factor analysis in neuropsychology. Psychological Record, 12, 147–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benton, A. L. (1967). Problems of test construction in the field of aphasia. Cortex, 3, 32–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luke, G. (2008). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 522–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chong, A., Sankar, L. & Poor, H. V. (2009). Frequency of occurrence and information entropy of American Sign Language, archived draft arXiv:0912.1768; downloaded from http://arxiv.org/abs/0912.1768

  • Cohen, M. J., Morgan, A. M., Vaughn, M., Riccio, C. A., & Hall, J. (1999). Verbal fluency in children: Developmental issues and differential validity in distinguishing children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and two subtypes of dyslexia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(5), 433–443.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corina, D., & Emmorey, K. (1993, November). Lexical priming in American Sign Language.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A. (2005). Lexical access in bilingual production. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 308–328). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, P. S. R., Gao, F. Q., Mason, W. P., Winocur, G., & Anderson, N. D. (2008). Verbal fluency, trail making, and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance following right frontal lobe tumor resection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(1), 18–32. doi:10.1080/13803390601161166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B. (1996). A theory and model of executive function: A neuropsychological perspective. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 263–278). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K., McCullough, S., & Brentari, D. (2003). Categorical perception in American Sign Language. Language & Cognitive Processes, 18, 21–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grogan, A., Green, D. W., Ali, N., Crinion, J. T., & Price, C. (2009). Structural correlates of semantic and phonemic fluency ability in first and second languages. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 2690–2698. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halperin, J. M., Healey, J. M., Zeitchik, E., Ludman, W. L., & Weinstein, L. (1989). Developmental aspects of linguistic and mnestic abilities in normal children. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 518–528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, M., & Slowiaczek, L. M. (1996). Phonological priming reflects lexical competition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 520–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance in traumatic brain injured patients. Neuropsychology, 18, 621–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempler, D., Teng, E. L., Dick, M., Taussig, I. M., & Davis, D. S. (1998). The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal fluency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 531–538.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loonstra, A. S., Tarlow, A. R., & Sellers, A. H. (2001). COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Applied Neuropsychology, 8(3), 161–166. doi:10.1207/S15324826AN0803_5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 29–41. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marschark, M., & Mayer, T. S. (1998). Interactions of language and memory in deaf children and adults. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 145–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D’Elia, L. F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morford, J. P., & Macfarlane, J. (2003). Frequency characteristics of American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies, 3(2), 213–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morford, J. P., Wilkinson, E., Villwock, A., Piñar, P., & Kroll, J. F. (2011). When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations? Cognition, 118(2), 286–292. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.). (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Portocarrero, J. S., Burright, R. G., & Donovick, P. J. (2007). Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 415–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raboutet, C., Sauzeon, H., Corsini, M., Rodrigques, J., Langevin, S., & N’Kaoua, B. (2010). Performance on semantic verbal fluency task across time: Dissociation between clustering, switching, and categorical exploitation process. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 268–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Regard, M., Strauss, E., & Knapp, P. (1982). Children’s production on verbal and non-verbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55(3), 839–844.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rende, B., Ramsberger, G., & Miyake, A. (2002). Commonalities and differences in the working memory components underlying letter and category fluency tasks: A dual-task investigation. Neuropsychology, 16(3), 309–321. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez-Aranda, C., & Martinussen, M. (2006). Age-related differences in performance of phonemic verbal fluency measured by Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT): a meta-analytic study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(2), 697–717.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., & Parker, S. B. (1996). Benton controlled oral word association test: Reliability and updated norms. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11(4), 329–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., & Levin, H. S. (1997). The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and Language, 57, 394–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S., Baldo, J., Graves, R. E., & Brugger, P. (2003). Pervasive influence of semantics in letter and category fluency: A multidimensional approach. Brain and Language, 87(3), 400–411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spreen, O., & Benton, A. L. (1969). Neurosensory center comprehensive examination for aphasia: Manual of directions. Victoria, BC: Neuropsychology Laboratory, University of Victoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure. Studies in linguistics. Buffalo, NY: University of Buffalo Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 131–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1997). Working memory for sign language: A window into the architecture of the working memory system. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(3), 121–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. R., Beitchman, J. H., Johnson, C., Douglas, L., Atkinson, L., Escobar, M., et al. (2002). Young adult academic outcomes in a longitudinal sample of early identified language impaired and control children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 635–645. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Donna A. Morere .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Morere, D.A., Witkin, G., Murphy, L. (2012). Measures of Expressive Language. In: Morere, D., Allen, T. (eds) Assessing Literacy in Deaf Individuals. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5269-0_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics