Skip to main content

Picoeconomics in Neural and Evolutionary Contexts

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Social Neuroscience and Public Health

Abstract

Hyperbolic delay discount curves reflect a basic psychophysical principle and are not maladaptive in nonhumans. However, in people who plan, they create conflicts between present motives and expected future motives. Unlike conflicts between simultaneous motives, these cannot be resolved by simply weighing the alternatives against one another, but instead confront a person with sequential strategic choices. Such choices are the subject of picoeconomics (micro–micro-economics). In recent centuries willpower has become the most approved means of stabilizing intertemporal conflicts, in addition to social commitment. In willpower a variant of repeated prisoner’s dilemma can be inferred from behavioral experiments and common experience—as clarified by thought experiments—but current neuroimaging techniques cannot visualize the self-interpretations that are hypothesized. fMRI does suggest that a unified reward network is modulated by prefrontal cortical activity, which is recruited even by the process of choice itself.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Agarwal, D. P., & Goedde, H. W. (1989). Human aldehyde dehydrogenases: Their role in alcoholism. Alcohol, 6, 517–523.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (1974). Impulse control in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 485–489.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463–496.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (1991). Derivation of “rational” economic behavior from hyperbolic discount curves. American Economic Review, 81, 334–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (1992). Picoeconomics: The strategic interaction of successive motivational states within the person. Cambridge: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2001). Breakdown of will. New York: Cambridge University.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2005). Précis of breakdown of will. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 635–673.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2006). Motivation must be momentary. In J. Elster, O. Gjelsvik, A. Hylland & K Moene (Eds.), Understanding choice, explaining behaviour: Essays in honour of Ole-Jorgen Skog (pp. 11–28). Oslo: Unipub Forlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2007). Can thought experiments prove anything about the will? In D. Spurrett, D. Ross, H. Kincaid & L. Stephens (Eds.), Distributed cognition and the will: Individual volition and social context (pp. 169–196). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2010). Procrastination, the basic impulse. In C. Andreou & M. White (Eds.), The thief of time: Philosophical essays on procrastination (pp. 11–27). New York, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2011). Free will as recursive self-prediction: Does a deterministic mechanism reduce responsibility? In J. Poland &G. Graham (Eds.), Addiction and Responsibility (pp. 55–87). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2012). Pure hyperbolic discount curves predict “eyes open” self-control. Theory and Decision, 73, 3–34. doi:10.1007/s11238-011-9272-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (in press). Grasping the impalpable: The role of endogenous reward in process addictions. Inquiry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1981). Preference reversal and delayed reinforcement. Animal Learning and Behavior 9, 476–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G., Haendel, V. (1983). The motives of the will. In E. Gottheil, K. Druley, T. Skodola & H. Waxman (Eds.), Etiology aspects of alcohol and drug abuse (pp. 119–140). Springfield, Ill.: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G., & Monterosso, J. (2003). Building blocks of self-control: Increased tolerance for delay with bundled rewards. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 79, 83–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, G. A. (1978). Perspectives on the history of psychoactive substance abuse. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angeletos, G.-M., Laibson, D., Repetto, A., Tobacman, J., & Weinberg, S. (2001). The hyperbolic consumption model: Calibration, simulation, and empirical evaluation. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 15, 47–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2001). Planning in 3-year-olds: A reflection of the future self? In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental perspectives Erlbaum. UK: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55, 30–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C. (2003). Pleasures of the brain. Brain and Cognition, 52, 106–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. (2009). Wanting and liking: Observations from the neuroscience and psychology laboratory. Inquiry, 52, 378–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction, 96, 73–86.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, J. M., Momenan, R., & Hommer, D. W. (2009). Delay discounting correlates with proportional lateral frontal cortex volumes. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 710–713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A. J. (2003). Disgust discussed. Annals of Neurology, 53, 427–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. (1981). The role of guilt in the formation of modern society: England, 1350–1800. British Journal of Sociology, 32, 459–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, R., McKell, M., Justin, R., & Huettel, S. A. (2010). Functional neuroimaging of intertemporal choice models: A review. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 3(1), 27–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, C. D., Garavan, H., & Bellgrove, M. A. (2009). Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 631–646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christakou, A., Brammer, M., & Rubia, K. (2011). Maturation of limbic corticostriatal activation and connectivity associated with developmental changes in temporal discounting. Neuroimage, 54, 1344–1354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, S., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1967). Choice and delay of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 67–74.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cropper, M. L., Aydede, S. K., & Portney, P. R. (1992). Rates of time preference for saving lives. American Economic Review, 82, 469–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deluty, M. Z., Whitehouse, W. G., Millitz, M., & Hineline, P. (1983). Self-control and commitment involving aversive events. Behavioral Analysis Letters 3, 213–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diekhof, E. K., & Gruber, O. (2010). When desire collides with reason: Functional interactions between anteroventral prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens underlie the human ability to resist impulsive desires. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1488–1493.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • DuBreuil, B. (2009). Paleolithic public goods games: The evolution of brain and cooperation in Mid-Pleistocene hominins. http://african.cyberlogic.net/bdubreuil/pdf/PPGG.pdf.

  • Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Powers (Eds.), Handbook of emotion and cognition. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Figner, B., Knoch, D., Johnson, E. J., Krosch, A. R., Lisanby, S. H., Fehr, E., et al. (2010). Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 538–539.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 351–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1920/1956). Beyond the pleasure principle. In J. Strachey & A. Freud (Eds.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund freud (Vol. 18). London: Hogarth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women’s conceptions of self and morality. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 481–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glimcher, P. W. (2009). Choice: towards a standard back-pocket model. In P. W. Glimcher, C. Camerer, R. A. Poldrack, & E. Fehr (Eds.), Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain (pp. 503–521). NY: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., Fry, A., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., Myerson, J., & Macaux, E. W. (2005). Temporal discounting when the choice is between two delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1121–1133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324, 646–648.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. (1961). Relative and absolute strengths of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 267–272.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeyr, A., Ainslie, G., Charlton, R., & Ross, D. (2010). The relationship between addiction and reward bundling: An experiment comparing smokers and non-smokers. Addiction, 106, 402–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holton, R. (2009). Determinism, self-efficacy, and the phenomenology of free will. Inquiry, 52, 412–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1625–1633.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., & Griffiths, M. D. (2011). The role of structural characteristics in problematic video game play: An empirical study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 9, 320–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future: Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 54–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N. (2006). The present values of delayed rewards are approximately additive. Behavioural Processes, 72, 273–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N., & Guastello, B. (2001). Making choices in anticipation of similar future choices can increase self-control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 154–164.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N., & Marakovic, N. N. (1995). Modeling myopic decisions: Evidence for hyperbolic delay-discounting within subjects and amounts. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 64, 22–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kober, H., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Kross, E. F., Weber, J., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., et al. (2010). Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 14811–14816.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1994). Motivation and volition. In G. d’Ydewalle, P. Bertelson & Eelen (Eds.), International perspectives on psychological science (Vol. 2, pp. 311–340). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laibson, D. (1997). Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 62, 443–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, S., Giragosian, L., Ainslie, G., Monterosso, J. (2009). Behavioral and neural evidence of incentive bias for immediate rewards relative to preference-matched delayed rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(47), 14820–14827. PMCID: PMC2821568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luo, S., Ainslie, G., Pollini, D., Giragosian, L., & Monterosso, J. R. (2011). Moderators of the association between brain activation and farsighted choice. Neuroimage,. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (1980). Determinants of relapse: Implications for the maintenance of behavior change. In P. O. Davidson & S. M. Davidson (Eds.), Behavioral medicine: Changing health lifestyles (pp. 410–452) Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E. (1986). Choice between single and multiple delayed reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 67–77.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E. (1997). Choice, delay, probability, and conditioned reinforcement. Animal Learning and Behavior, 25, 131–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E., & Biondi, D. R. (2009). Delay-amount tradeoffs in choices by pigeons and rates: Hyperbolic versus exponential discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91(2), 197–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The grasshopper and the ant: Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306, 503–507.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 5796–5804.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, J. P., Schirmer, J., Ames, D. L., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). Medial prefrontal cortex predicts intertemporal choice. Jounal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 857–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monterosso, J., & Ainslie, G. (1999). Beyond Discounting: Possible experimental models of impulse control. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 146, 339–347.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Monterosso, J. R., Ainslie, G., Toppi- Mullen, P., & Gault, B. (2002). The fragility of cooperation: A false feedback study of a sequential iterated prisoner’s dilemma. Journal of Economic Psychology, 23(4), 437–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monterosso, J. R., & Luo, S. (2010). An argument against dual valuation system competition: Cognitive capacities supporting future orientation mediate rather than compete with visceral motivations. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 3, 1–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monterosso, J., Mann, T., Ward, A., Ainslie, G., Bramen, J., Brody, A., & London, E. D. (2010). Neural recruitment during self-control of smoking: A pilot fMRI study. In D. Ross, H. Kincaid, D. Spurrett & P. Collins (Eds.), What is addiction (pp. 269–289). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2006). Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312, 1038–1040.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 419–427.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paserman, M. D. (2008). Job search and hyperbolic discounting: Structural estimation and policy evaluation. The Economic Journal, 118, 1418–1452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W., & Jasper, H. (1954). Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the human brain. Oxford, England: Little, Brown & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 66, 138–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Platt, M. L., & Padoa-Schioppa, C. (2009). Neuronal representations of value. In P. W. Glimcher, C. Camerer, R. A. Poldrack & E. Fehr (Eds.), Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain (pp. 441–462). NY: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1985). Dieting and binging: a causal analysis. American Psychologist, 40, 193–201.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (2008). Intangibility in intertemporal choice. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3813–3824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. T. (2005). Emotion explained. Oxford: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, D. (2010). Economic models of pathological gambling. In D. Ross, H. Kincaid, D. Spurrett & P. Collins (Eds.), What is addiction (pp. 131–158). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Nature and autonomy: An organizational view of social and neurobiological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 701–728.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. Review of Economic Studies, 4, 155–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (in press). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shea, D. B., Jr. (1968). Spiritual autobiography in Early America. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. B., Dugan, S., & Trompenaars, F. (1997). Locus of control and affectivity by gender and occupational status: A 14 nation study. Sex Roles, 36, 51–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R. W. (1984). Consciousness, personal identity and the divided brain. Neuropsychologia, 22, 661–673.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. N. (2009). A common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, L. (1977). The family, sex, and marriage: England, 1500–1800. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strotz, R. H. (1956). Myopia and inconsistency in dynamic utility maximization. Review of Economic Studies, 23, 166–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Telser, L. G. (1980). A theory of self-enforcing agreements. Journal of Business, 53, 27–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. (1904/1958). The protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. New York: Charles Scribners Sons.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to John Monterosso and to Shan Luo for comments and suggestions. This material is the result of work supported with resources and the use of facilities at the Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Coatesville, PA, USA. The opinions expressed are not those of the Department of Veterans Affairs or of the US Government.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to George Ainslie .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Highlights

Highlights

  • Human motivational conflict is best analyzed in the relationship between present and expected future selves, rather than between separate motivational centers.

  • People have inherited a delay discount curve that is probably a pure hyperbola, making us prone to addictions and impulsive behaviors.

  • The motivational force of willpower comes from seeing a current choice as a test case that predicts future choices in similar cases (recursive self-prediction).

  • Current neuroimaging techniques can reveal the interaction of motivational centers in self-control, but not their semantic content, such as the hypothesized recursive self-prediction.

  • Hyperbolic discount curves have survived in evolution because they have a deeply rooted psychophysical form, and are harmless in species whose future planning is instinctive.

  • There is no dimension of impulse control that is best maximized, since the major available strategies, social pressure and willpower, both have serious limitations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ainslie, G. (2013). Picoeconomics in Neural and Evolutionary Contexts. In: Hall, P. (eds) Social Neuroscience and Public Health. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6852-3_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6852-3_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4614-6851-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4614-6852-3

  • eBook Packages: MedicineMedicine (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics